Cultural Transmission Theory and the Archaeological Record: Providing Context to Understanding Variation and Temporal Changes in Material Culture (original) (raw)
References cited
Aldenderfer, M. A. (1993). Ritual, hierarchy, and change in foraging societies. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology12: 1–40. Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1975). Prestige and culture: A biosocial interpretation. Current Anthropology16: 553–576. Google Scholar
Barth, F. (1987). Cosmologies in the Making: A Generative Approach to Cultural Variation in Inner New Guinea, Cambridge University Press, New York. Google Scholar
Barth, F. (1990). The guru and the conjurer: Transactions in knowledge and the shaping of culture in Southeast Asia and Melanesia. Man25: 640–653. Google Scholar
Basalla, G. (1988). The Evolution of Technology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A. (2005). Academic copying, archaeology and the English language. Antiquity80: 196–201. Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A., and Maschner, H. D. G. (2000). A growing network of ideas. Fractals8: 227–238. Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A., and Maschner, H. D. G. (2001). Stylistic change as a self-organized critical phenomenon: An archaeological study in complexity. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory8: 35–65. Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A., and Shennan, S. J. (2003). Cultural transmission and stochastic network growth. American Antiquity68: 459–485. Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W., and Shennan, S. J. (2004). Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences271: 1443–1450. Google Scholar
Bettinger, R. L., and Eerkens, J. W. (1997). Evolutionary implications of metrical variation in Great Basin projectile points. In Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (eds.), Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological Explanation, Archeological Papers No. 7, American Anthropological Association, Arlington, VA, pp. 177–191. Google Scholar
Bettinger, R. L., and Eerkens, J. W. (1999). Point typologies, cultural transmission, and the spread of bow-and-arrow technology in the prehistoric Great Basin. American Antiquity64: 231–242. Google Scholar
Bettinger, R. L., Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1996). Style, function, and cultural evolutionary processes. In Maschner, H. D. G. (ed.), Darwinian Archaeologies, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 133–164. Google Scholar
Bikhchandani, S., Hirschleifer, D., and Welch, I. (1998). Learning from the behaviour of others: Conformity, fads and informational cascades. Journal of Economic Perspectives12: 151–170. Google Scholar
Biro, D., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., Yamakoshi, G., Sousa, C., and Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: Evidence from field experiments. Animal Cognition6: 213–223. Google Scholar
Blackham, M. (2000). Distinguishing bioturbation and trampling using pottery sherd measures, Tell Fendi, Jordan. Geoarchaeology15: 469–497. Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1896). The limitations of the comparative method in anthropology. Science4: 901–907. Google Scholar
Bonner, J. T. (1980). The Evolution of Culture in Animals, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process, University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of ethnic markers. Cultural Anthropology2: 65–79. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1988). An evolutionary model of social learning: The effects of spatial and temporal variation. In Zentall, T., and Galef, B. G. (eds.), Social Learning: A Psychological and Biological Approach, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 29–48. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology16: 125–143. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (2002). Group beneficial norms can spread rapidly in a structured population. Journal of Theoretical Biology215: 287–296. Google Scholar
Brew, J. O. (1946). Archaeology of Alkali Ridge, Southeastern Utah, Papers of the Peabody Museum of American Archaeology and Ethnology No. 21, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.
Brown, C., and Laland, K. N. (2003). Social learning in fishes: A review. Fish and Fisheries4: 280–288. Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1965). Variation and selective retention in socio-cultural evolution. In Barringer, H. R., Blanksten, G. I., and Mack, R. W. (eds.), Social Change in Developing Areas: A Reinterpretation of Evolutionary Theory, Schenkman, Cambridge, MA, pp. 19–49. Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1973). Models for cultural inheritance. I. Group mean and within group variation. Theoretical Population Biology4: 42–55. Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1983). Paradox of the evolution of communication and of social interactivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences80: 2017–2021. Google Scholar
Cloak, F. T., Jr. (1975). Is a cultural ethology possible? Human Ecology3: 161–182. Google Scholar
Combet, F., Jaussaud, P., and Martin, N. (2005). Estimation of slight speed gaps between signals via the scale transform. Mechanical Systems & Signal Processing19: 230–257. Google Scholar
Conkey, M., and Hastorf, C. (eds.) (1990). The Use of Style in Archaeology, University of Cambridge Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar
Coren, S. L., Ward, L. M., and Enns, J. T. (1994). Sensation and Perception, Harcourt Brace, Fort Worth, TX.
Cover, T. M., and Thomas, J. A. (1991). Elements of Information Theory, Wiley, New York. Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1999). That Complex Whole: Culture and the Evolution of Human Behavior, Westview, Boulder, CO. Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene, Oxford University Press, New York. Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype, Oxford University Press, Oxford. Google Scholar
De Winter, W. (1997). The beanbag genetics controversy: Towards a synthesis of opposing views of natural selection. Biology and Philosophy12: 149–184. Google Scholar
Deetz, J. (1965). The Dynamics of Stylistic Change in Arikara Ceramics, Illinois Studies in Anthropology, Urbana.
Deetz, J., and Dethlefsen, E. S. (1965). The Doppler-effect and archaeology: A consideration of the spatial aspects of seriation. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology21: 196–206. Google Scholar
Deetz, J., and Dethlefsen, E. S. (1971). Some social aspects of New England colonial mortuary art. In Brown, J. A. (ed.), Memoirs of the Society for American Archaeology, No. 25, Society for American Archaeology, Washington, DC, pp. 30–38. Google Scholar
Dobres, M. A. (2000). Creativity in human evolution and prehistory. American Antiquity65: 768–769. Google Scholar
Doolittle, W. F., and Sapienza, C. (1980). Selfish genes, the phenotype paradigm and genome evolution. Nature284: 601–603. Google Scholar
Dunnell, R. C. (1971). Systematics in Prehistory, Free Press, New York. Google Scholar
Dunnell, R. C. (1978). Style and function: A fundamental dichotomy. American Antiquity43: 192–202. Google Scholar
Dunnell, R. C. (1985). Methodological issues in contemporary Americanist archaeology. In Asquith, P. D., and Ketcher, P. (eds.), Proceedings of the 1984 Biennial Meetings of the Philosophy of Science Association, Volume 2, Philosophy of Science Association, East Lansing, MI, pp. 717–744.
Dunnell, R. C. (1992). Archaeology and evolutionary science. In Wandsnider, L. (ed.), Quandaries and Quests: Visions of Archaeology’s Future, Occasional Paper No. 20, Center for Archaeological Investigations, Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, pp. 209–221.
Durham, W. H. (1976). The adaptive significance of cultural behavior. Human Ecology4: 89–121. Google Scholar
Eerkens, J. W. (2000). Practice makes within 5% of perfect: The role of visual perception, motor skills, and human memory in artifact variation and standardization. Current Anthropology41: 663–668. Google Scholar
Eerkens, J. W., and Bettinger, R. L. (2001). Techniques for assessing standardization in artifact assemblages: Can we scale material variability? American Antiquity66: 493–504. Google Scholar
Eerkens, J. W., and Lipo, C. P. (2005). Cultural transmission, copying errors, and the generation of variation in material culture in the archaeological record. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology24: 316–334. Google Scholar
Feathers, J. K. (1989). Ceramic analysis, variation, and database construction: A selectionist perspective. In Blakely, J. A., and Bennett, W. J. J. (eds.), Analysis and Publication of Ceramics: The Computer Database in Archaeology, BAR International Series 551, British Archaeological Reports, Oxford, pp. 71–80.
Feathers, J. K. (1990). Explaining the Evolution of Ceramics in Southeast Missouri, Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, Seattle.
Feldman, M. W., Aoki, K., and Kumm, J. (1996). Individual versus social learning: Evolutionary analysis in a fluctuating environment. Anthropological Science104: 209–232. Google Scholar
Ford, J. A. (1935). Ceramic Decoration Sequence at an Old Indian Village Site, Near Sicily Island, Louisiana, Anthropological Study No. 1, Department of Conservation, Louisiana Geological Survey, New Orleans.
Ford, J. A. (1936). Analysis of Indian Village Site Collections from Louisiana and Mississippi, Anthropological Study No. 2, Department of Conservation, Louisiana Geological Survey, New Orleans.
Ford, J. K. B. (1991). Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology69: 1454–1483. Article Google Scholar
Fritz, J., and Kotraschal, K. (1999). Social learning in common ravens (Corvus corax). Animal Behavior57: 785–793. Google Scholar
Fromkin, V., and Rodman, R. (1997). An Introduction to Language, 6th ed., Harcourt Brace, London. Google Scholar
Gabora, L. M. (2000). The beer can theory of creativity. In Bentley, P., and Corne, D. (eds.), Creative Evolutionary Systems, Morgan Kaufman, San Francisco, pp. 1–26. Google Scholar
Gabora, L. M. (2004). Ideas are not replicators but minds are. Biology and Philosophy19: 127–143. Google Scholar
Gell-Mann, M., and Lloyd, S. (2003). Effective Complexity, Santa Fe Institute Working Paper 03-12-068, Santa Fe, NM.
Gibbs, W. W. (2003). The unseen genome: Gems among the junk. Scientific American289: 46–53. Article Google Scholar
Grant, P., and Grant, R. (1996). Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin’s finches. Evolution50: 2471–2487. Google Scholar
Hahn, M. W., and Bentley, R. A. (2003). Drift as a mechanism for cultural change: An example from baby names. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biology LettersB270: S120–S123.
Harpole, J., and Lyman, R. L. (2002). Changes in the fashion of women’s bonnets, 1831–1895. Missouri Archaeologist63: 21–30. Google Scholar
Hatch, E. (1973). Theories of Man and Culture, Columbia University Press, New York. Google Scholar
Hayden, B. (ed.) (1979). Lithic Use-Wear Analysis, Academic Press, New York. Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: Adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominant force in behavioral change. American Anthropologist103: 992–1013. Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004a). Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization53: 3–35. Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004b). Demography and cultural evolution: Why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. American Antiquity69: 197–221. Google Scholar
Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior19: 215–242. Google Scholar
Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (2001). Why people punish defectors: Conformist transmission stabilizes costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology208: 79–89. Google Scholar
Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (2002). Culture and cognition: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations. Culture and Cognition2: 87–112. Google Scholar
Henrich, J., and Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior22: 165–196. Google Scholar
Henrich, J., and McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Archaeology12: 123–135. Google Scholar
Herzog, H. A., Bentley, R. A., and Hahn, M. W. (2004). Random drift and large shifts in popularity of dog breeds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences271: S353–S356. Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M., and Galef, J. B. G. (eds.) (1996). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, Academic Press, London. Google Scholar
Hill, J. N. (1966). A prehistoric community in eastern Arizona. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology22: 9–30. Google Scholar
Hill, J. N. (1968). Broken K Pueblo: Patterns of form and function. In Binford, S. R., and Binford, L. R. (eds.), New Perspectives in Archeology, Aldine, Chicago, pp. 103–142. Google Scholar
Holmes, W. H. (1886). Ancient Pottery of the Mississippi Valley, Fourth Annual Report of the Bureau of Ethnology, 1882–83, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC, pp. 361–436. Google Scholar
Houston, S., Stuart, D., and Robertson, J. (1998). Disharmony in Maya hieroglyphic writing: Linguistic change and continuity in Classic society. In Ciudad Ruiz, A., Fernandez Marquinez, Y., Garcia Campillo, J. M., Iglesias Ponce de Leon, M. J., Garcia-Gallo, A. L., and Sanz Castro, L. T. (eds.), Anatomía de una civilización: aproximaciones interdisciplinarias a la cultura maya, Sociedad Española de Estudios Mayas, Madrid, pp. 275–296. Google Scholar
Hruby, Z. X. (2002). Evidence for linguistic conservatism in the hieroglyphic script of the central Peten. Mayab15: 49–59. Google Scholar
Jordan, P., and Shennan, S. (2003). Cultural transmission, language, and basketry traditions amongst the California Indians. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology22: 42–74. Google Scholar
Kidder, A. V. (1915). Pottery of the Pajarito Plateau and some adjacent regions in New Mexico. In Memoirs of the American Anthropological Association, Vol. 2, American Anthropological Association, Lancaster, pp. 407–462.
Kidder, A. V. (1917). A design-sequence from New Mexico. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences3: 369–370.
Kohler, T. A., VanBuskirk, S., and Ruscavage-Barz, S. (2004). Vessels and villages: Evidence for conformist transmission in early village aggregations on the Pajarito Plateau, New Mexico. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology23: 100–118. Google Scholar
Koppers, W. (1955). Diffusion: Transmission and acceptance. Yearbook of Anthropology1955: 169–181. Google Scholar
Kornbacher, K. (2001). Building components of evolutionary explanation: A study of wedge tools from northern South America. In Hunt, T., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 23–72. Google Scholar
Krieger, A. D. (1944). The typological concept. American Antiquity3: 271–288. Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L. (1916). Zuni Potsherds, Anthropological Paper No. 18, Part 1, American Museum of Natural History, New York.
Kroeber, A. L. (1919). On the principle of order in civilization as exemplified by changes of fashion. American Anthropologist21: 235–263. Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L. (1940). Statistical classification. American Antiquity6: 29–44. Google Scholar
Krützen, M., Mann, J., Heithaus, M. R., Connor, R. C., Bejder, L., and Sherwin, W. B. (2005). Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences102: 8939–8943. Google Scholar
Kuijt, I. (2000). People and space in early agricultural villages: Exploring daily lives, community size and architecture in the late pre-pottery Neolithic. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology19: 75–102. Google Scholar
Kujit, I. (2001). Place, death, and the transmission of social memory in early agricultural communities of the Near Eastern Pre-Pottery Neolithic. In Chesso, M. S. (ed.), Social Memory, Identity, and Death: Intradisciplinary Perspectives on Mortuary Rituals, American Anthropological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 80–99. Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). The social motivation of a sound change. In Labov, W. (ed.), Sociolinguistic Patterns, University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, pp. 1–42. Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1994). Principles of Linguistic Change: Internal Factors, Basil Blackwell, Oxford. Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. (1974). The Genetic Basis of Evolutionary Change, Columbia University Press, New York. Google Scholar
Lieberson, S. (2000). A Matter of Taste: How Names, Fashions, and Culture Change, Yale University Press, New Haven, CT. Google Scholar
Lipo, C. (2001a). Science, Style and the Study of Community Structure: An Example from the Central Mississippi River Valley, BAR International Series, No. 918, British Archaeological Reports, Oxford.
Lipo, C. (2001b). Community structures among late Mississippian populations of the central Mississippi River valley. In Hunt, T., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 175–216 Google Scholar
Lipo, C., and Madsen, M. (2000). Neutrality, “style,” and drift: Building methods for studying cultural transmission in the archaeological record. In Hurt, T. D., and Rakita, G. F. M. (eds.), Style and Function: Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 91–118. Google Scholar
Lipo, C., Madsen, M., Dunnell, R., and Hunt, T. (1997). Population structure, cultural transmission, and frequency seriation. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology16: 301–333. Google Scholar
Longacre, W. A. (1970). Archaeology as Anthropology: A Case Study, University of Arizona Press, Tucson. Google Scholar
Lumsden, C. J., and Wilson, E. O. (1981). Genes, Mind and Culture, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L. (2000). Culture historical and biological approaches to identifying homologous traits. In Hurt, T. D., and Rakita, G. F. M. (eds.), Style and Function. Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 69–89. Google Scholar
Lyman, R., and O’Brien, M. (2000). Measuring and explaining change in artifact variation with clade-diversity diagrams. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology19: 39–74. Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L., and O’Brien, M. J. (2003). Cultural traits: Units of analysis in early twentieth-century anthropology. Journal of Anthropological Research59: 225–250. Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L., O’Brien, M. J., and Dunnell, R. C. (1997). The Rise and Fall of Culture History, Plenum Press, New York. Google Scholar
Lynch, A. (1996). The population memetics of birdsong. In Kroodsma, D. E., and Miller, E. H. (eds.), Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, pp. 181–197. Google Scholar
MacDonald, D. (1998). Subsistence, sex, and cultural transmission in Folsom culture. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology17: 217–239. Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1959). Where are we. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium in Quantitative Biology24: 1–14. Google Scholar
McBrearty, S., Bishop, L., Plummer, T., Dewar, R., and Conard, N. (1998). Tools underfoot: Human trampling as an agent of lithic artifact edge modification. American Antiquity63: 108–129. Google Scholar
McClure, S. B. (2004). Cultural Transmission of Ceramic Technology During the Consolidation of Agriculture in Valencia, Spain, Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of California, Santa Barbara, University Microfilms, Ann Arbor.
McElreath, R., Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology44: 122–129. Google Scholar
McGrew, W. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture. Implications for Human Evolution, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar
Meggers, B., Evans, C., and Estrada, B. (1965). Early Formative Period of Coastal Ecuador: The Valdivia and Machalilla Phases, Smithsonian Contributions to Anthropology I, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC. Google Scholar
Meltzer, D. J. (1981). A study of style and function in a class of tools. Journal of Field Archaeology8: 313–326. Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., and Whiten, A. (2004). The hierarchical transformation of event knowledge in human cultural transmission. Journal of Cognition and Culture4: 1–24. Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology97: 405–423. Google Scholar
Mithen, S. J. (1997). Cognitive archaeology, evolutionary psychology, and cultural transmission with particular reference to religious ideas. In Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (eds.), Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary Theory in Archaeological Explanation, Archeological Paper No. 7, American Anthropological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 67–74.
Mithen, S. J. (1998). A creative explosion? Theory of mind, language, and the disembodied mind of the Upper Paleolithic. In Mithen, S. J. (ed.), Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory, Routledge, London, pp. 165–191. Google Scholar
Neiman, F. (1995). Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective: Inferences from decorative diversity and interassemblage distance in Illinois Woodland ceramic assemblages. American Antiquity60: 7–36. Google Scholar
Nelson, N. C. (1916). Chronology of the Tanos ruins, New Mexico. American Anthropologist18: 159–180. Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates9: 167–224. Google Scholar
Norwich, K. H. (1983). On the theory of Weber fractions. Perception and Psychophysics42: 286–298. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., and Holland, T. D. (1990). Variation, selection, and the archaeological record. In Schiffer, M. B. (ed.), Archaeological Method and Theory, Vol. 2, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 31–80. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (1999). Seriation, Stratigraphy, and Index Fossils: The Backbone of Archaeological Dating, Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press, New York. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2002). The epistemological nature of archaeological units. Anthropological Theory2: 37–56. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2003). Style, function, transmission: An introduction. In O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (eds.), Style, Function, Transmission: Evolutionary Archaeological Perspectives, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 1–32. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., Holland, T. D., Hoard, R. J., and Fox, G. L. (1994). Evolutionary implications of design and performance characteristics of prehistoric pottery. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory1: 259–304. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., Lyman, R. L., and Schiffer, M. B. (2005). Archaeology as a Process: Processualism and Its Progeny, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City. Google Scholar
Olsen, S. L., and Shipman, P. (1988). Surface modification on bone: Trampling versus butchery. Journal of Archaeological Science15: 535–553. Google Scholar
Orgel, L. E., and Crick, F. H. C. (1980). Selfish DNA: The ultimate parasite. Nature284: 604–607. Google Scholar
Osgood, C. (1951). Culture: Its empirical and non-empirical character. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology7: 202–214. Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., and Wright, S. A. (1997). Categories and gatherings: Group selection and the mythology of cultural anthropology. Evolution and Human Behavior18: 291–308. Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., Coe, K., and Wadley, R. L. (2005). On tools and tradition. Current Anthropology46: 459–460. Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., Frederickson, B. E., and Tilley, C. F. (1995). On cultural group selection. Current Anthropology36: 657–658. Google Scholar
Petrie, W. M. F. (1899). Sequences in prehistoric remains. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland29: 295–301. Google Scholar
Pfeffer, M. T. (2001). The engineering and evolution of Hawaiian fishhooks. In Hunt, T. L., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 73–96. Google Scholar
Phillips, P., Ford, J. A., and Griffin, J. B. (1951). Archaeological Survey in the Lower Mississippi Alluvial Valley, 1940–1947, Papers of the Peabody Museum of American Archaeology and Ethnology 25, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.
Pocklington, R., and Best, M. L. (1997). Cultural evolution and units of selection in replicating text. Journal of Theoretical Biology188: 79–87. Google Scholar
Poole, J. H., Tyack, P. L., Stoeger-Horwath, A. S., and Watwood, S. (2005). Animal behaviour: Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature434: 455–456. Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1959). The Logic of Scientific Discovery, Basic Books, New York. Google Scholar
Rendell, L., and Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences24: 309–382. Google Scholar
Rickerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (1978). A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process. Journal of Social and Biological Structures1: 127–154. Google Scholar
Rogers, E. M. (2003). The Diffusion of Innovations, 5th ed., Free Press, New York. Google Scholar
Rouse, I. (1939). Prehistory in Haiti, Yale University Publications in Anthropology No. 21, New Haven, CT.
Rowlands, M. (1993). The role of memory in the transmission of culture. World Archaeology25: 141–151. Article Google Scholar
Schiffer, M. B. (1987). Formation Processes of the Archaeological Record, University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque.
Schiffer, M. B. (2004). Memes are a bad idea: Behavioral approaches to cultural transmission and evolution. Paper presented at the 68th Annual Meeting for the Society for American Archaeology, Montreal.
Schiffer, M. B., and Skibo, J. M. (1997). The explanation of artifact variability. American Antiquity62: 27–50. Google Scholar
Schiffer, M. B., Skibo, J. M., Griffitts, J. L., Hollenback, K. L., and Longacre, W. A. (2001). Behavioral archaeology and the study of technology. American Antiquity66: 729–737. Google Scholar
Schlag, K. H. (1998). Why imitate, and if so, how? Journal of Economic Theory78: 130–156. Google Scholar
Shannon, C. E., and Weaver, W. (1949). The Mathematical Theory of Communication, University of Illinois Press, Urbana. Google Scholar
Sheng-Wei, L., Jin-Jang, L., and Li-Wei, K. (2004). An error resilient coding scheme for H.26L video transmission based on data embedding. Journal of Visual Communication and Image Representation15: 214–240. Google Scholar
Shennan, S. J. (1989). Cultural transmission and cultural change. In Van der Leeuw, S. E., and Torrance, R. (eds.), What’s New: A Closer Look at the Process of Innovation, Unwin Hyman, London, pp. 330–346. Google Scholar
Shennan, S. J. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal11: 5–16. Article Google Scholar
Shennan, S. J. (2002). Genes, Memes, and Human History: Darwinian Archaeology and Cultural Evolution, Thames and Hudson, London. Google Scholar
Shennan, S. J., and Wilkinson, J. R. (2001). Ceramic style change and neutral evolution: A case study from Neolithic Europe. American Antiquity66: 577–593. Google Scholar
Shott, M. J. (1997). Stones and shafts redux: The metric discrimination of chipped-stone dart and arrow points. American Antiquity62: 86–101. Google Scholar
Skibo, J. M. (1992). Pottery Function: A Use-Alteration Perspective, Plenum Press, New York. Google Scholar
Slater, P. J. B., and Ince, S. A. (1979). Cultural evolution in chaffinch song. Behavior71: 146–166. Google Scholar
Smith, J. M., and Bell, P. A. (1994). Conformity as a determinant of behavior in a resource dilemma. Journal of Social Psychology134: 191–200. Article Google Scholar
Soltis, J., Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1995). Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection? An empirical test. Current Anthropology36: 473–494. Google Scholar
Spencer, C. (1993). Human agency, biased transmission, and the cultural evolution of chiefly authority. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology12: 41–74. Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach, Blackwell, Oxford. Google Scholar
Spier, L. (1917). An Outline for a Chronology of Zuni Ruins, Anthropological Papers No. 18, Part 3, American Museum of Natural History, New York.
Tehrani, J., and Collard, M. (2002). Investigating cultural evolution through biological phylogenetic analyses of Turkmen textiles. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology21: 443–463. Google Scholar
Teltser, P. A. (1995). Culture history, evolutionary theory, and frequency seriation. In Teltser, P. A. (ed.), Evolutionary Archaeology: Methodological Issues, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 51–68 Google Scholar
Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (1992). Psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds.), The Adapted Mind, Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 19–136. Google Scholar
Trask, R. L. (1994). Language Change, Routledge, New York. Google Scholar
van Doorn, A. J., van de Grind, W. A., and Koenderink, J. J. (1984). Limits in Perception: Essays in Honour of Maarten A. Bouman, VNU Science Press, Utrecht, The Netherlands. Google Scholar
Wang, R., Hong, F., and Chen, Z. (2004). Cryptography with no transmission error or cipher-text extension based on orthogonal table. Mini-Micro Systems25: 1499–1501. Google Scholar
Washburn, D. K. (2001). Remembering things seen: Experimental approaches to the process of information transmittal. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory8: 67–99. Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (1992). Memorable religions: Transmission, codification and change in divergent Melanesian contexts. Man27: 777–799. Google Scholar
Wilhelmsen, K. H. (2001). Building the framework for an evolutionary explanation of projectile point variation: An example from the central Mississippi River valley. In Hunt, T. L., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 97–144. Google Scholar
Woese, C. R. (2000). Interpreting the universal phylogenetic tree. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences97: 8392–8396. Google Scholar
Woese, C. R. (2004). A new biology for a new century. Microbiology and Molecular Biology Reviews68: 173–186. Google Scholar
Wrangham, R., McGrew, W., de Waal, F., and Heltne, P. (1996). Chimpanzee Cultures, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Google Scholar
Bibliography of recent literature
Atran, S. (2001). The trouble with memes: Inference versus imitation in cultural creation. Human Nature12: 351–381. Google Scholar
Aunger, R. (2002). The Electric Meme: A New Theory of How We Think, Free Press, New York. Google Scholar
Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (1997). Evolutionary theory and archaeological explanation. In Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (eds.), Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological Explanation, Archeological Papers Vol. 7, American Anthropological Association, Arlington, VA, pp. 3–15.
Beck, C. (1998). Projectile point types as valid chronological units. In Ramenofsky, A. F., and Steffen, A. (eds.), Unit Issues in Archaeology: Measuring Time, Space, and Material, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 21–40. Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A. (2003). Cultural transmission and stochastic network growth. American Antiquity68: 459–485. Google Scholar
Best, M. L., and Pocklington, R. (1999). Meaning as use: Transmission fidelity and evolution in netnews. Journal of Theoretical Biology196: 389–395. Google Scholar
Boone, J. L., and Smith, E. A. (1998). Is it evolution yet? A critique of evolutionary archaeology. Current Anthropology39: 141–173. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of ethnic markers. Cultural Anthropology2: 65–79. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy88: 77–93. Google Scholar
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (2002). Group beneficial norms spread rapidly in a structured population. Journal of Theoretical Biology215: 287–296. Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1999). Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: How evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission. American Anthropologist100: 876–889. Google Scholar
Broughton, J. M., and O’Connell, J. F. (1999). On evolutionary ecology, selectionist archaeology, and behavioral archaeology. American Antiquity64: 153–165. Google Scholar
Darwent, J., and O’Brien, M. J., (2005). Using cladistics to construct lineages of projectile points from northeastern Missouri. In Lipo, C. L., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M., and Shennan, S. J. (eds.), Mapping Our Ancestors: Phylogenetic Approaches in Anthropology and Prehistory, Aldine, New York, pp. 185–208. Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: Adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change. American Anthropologist103: 992–1013. Google Scholar
Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (2002). Culture and cognition: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations. Culture and Cognition2: 87–112. Google Scholar
Jones, G. T, Leonard, R. D., and Abbot, A. L. (1995). The structure of selectionist explanations in archaeology. In Teltser, P. A. (ed.), Evolutionary Archaeology: Methodological Issues, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 13–32. Google Scholar
Lanata, J. L., and Neff, H. (1999). Discutiendo algunas escalas of transmision cultural: artefacto y espacio. Revista do Museo de Arquelogia y Etnologia (Sao Paulo), Supplemento3: 46–56. Google Scholar
Lipo, C. L., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M., and Shennan, S. J. (eds.) (2005). Mapping Our Ancestors: Phylogenetic Approaches in Anthropology and Prehistory, Aldine, New York. Google Scholar
Lynch, A. (1996). Thought Contagion: How Belief Spreads Through Society, Basic Books, New York. Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., and Laland, K. N. (2004). Perspective: Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of the Origin of Species. Evolutionary Anthropology58: 1–11. Google Scholar
Neff, H. (2000). On evolutionary ecology and evolutionary archaeology: Some common ground? Current Anthropology41: 427–429. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2000). Measuring and explaining change in artifact variation with clade-diversity diagrams. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology19: 39–74. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2004). History and explanation in archaeology. Anthropological Theory4: 173–197. Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., Darwent, J., and Lyman, R. L. (2001). Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Paleoindian points from the southeastern United States. Journal of Archaeological Science28: 1115–1136. Google Scholar
Pierce, C. (1998). Theory, measurement, and explanation, variable shapes in poverty point objects. In Ramenofsky, A. F., and Steffen, A. (eds.), Unit Issues in Archaeology: Measuring Time, Space, and Material, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 163–190. Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., and Henrich, J. (2003). The cultural evolution of human cooperation. In Hammerstein, P. (ed.), The Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 357–388. Google Scholar
Shennan, S. J. (2002). Learning. In Hart, J. P., and Terrell, J. E. (eds.), Darwin and Archaeology: A Handbook of Key Concepts, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 183–200. Google Scholar
Taylor, G. (1996). Cultural Selection, BasicBooks, New York. Google Scholar
Tschauner, H. (1994). Archaeological systematics and cultural evolution: Retrieving the honour of culture history. Man29: 77–93. Google Scholar