A Local, Bottom-Up Perspective on Sleep Deprivation and Neurobehavioral Performance (original) (raw)

. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Sep 1.

Published in final edited form as: Curr Top Med Chem. 2011 Sep 1;11(19):2414–2422. doi: 10.2174/156802611797470286

Abstract

Waking neurobehavioral performance is temporally regulated by a sleep/wake homeostatic process and a circadian process in interaction with a time-on-task effect. Neurobehavioral impairment resulting from these factors is task-specific, and characterized by performance variability. Several aspects of these phenomena are not well understood, and cannot be explained solely by a top-down (subcortically driven) view of sleep/wake and performance regulation. We present a bottom-up theory, where we postulate that task performance is degraded by local, use-dependent sleep in neuronal groups subserving cognitive processes associated with the task at hand. The theory offers explanations for the temporal dependence of neurobehavioral performance on time awake, time on task, and their interaction; for the effectiveness of task switching and rest breaks to overcome the time-on-task effect (but not the effects of sleep deprivation); for the task-specific nature of neurobehavioral impairment; and for the stochastic property of performance variability.

Keywords: Use-dependence, cognition, time on task, state instability, sleep homeostasis, near-infrared optical imaging, theory development

TEMPORAL REGULATION OF NEUROBEHAVIORAL PERFORMANCE

It has long been recognized that human waking functions change dynamically over time across a wide range of cognitive, motor, and behavioral domains – including task performance accuracy, reaction time, decision making, emotional reactivity, subjective sleepiness, etc. Describing these various domains collectively with the term neurobehavioral performance, their combined temporal dynamics over hours and days are regulated by two key biological processes: a homeostatic process that keeps track of time awake and time asleep, and a circadian process that keeps track of time of day [13].

The homeostatic process builds up a pressure for sleep over time awake that results in neurobehavioral performance degradation during periods of sleep deprivation or sleep restriction [46]. Conversely, the pressure for sleep is dissipated across time spent asleep, so that “recovery” sleep or even a brief nap results in improvement of previously degraded neurobehavioral performance [5, 79]. Concurrently, the circadian process modulates neurobehavioral performance over the 24 hours of the day, leading to relative improvement during the biological day and relative impairment during the biological night [10, 11].

In interaction, the homeostatic and circadian processes affect neurobehavioral performance as a function of the timing and duration of the waking periods in ways that are easily explained for conditions of total sleep deprivation – see Fig. (1) - but get more complicated for schedules in which the waking periods are temporally displaced (e.g., as tends to be the case for shift workers). To deal with this issue, mathematical models have been developed to predict neurobehavioral performance changes over time based on the dynamics of the homeostatic and circadian processes [1214].

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Homeostatic and circadian regulation of neurobehavioral performance during total sleep deprivation. The left panel shows the gradual increase of pressure for sleep from the homeostatic process across 62 hours of total sleep deprivation, and the modulatory influence of the circadian process with an oscillatory period of 24 hours. The right panel shows the sum of these two biological processes as a model of neurobehavioral performance impairment (gray curve), superimposed on actual measurements of performance in the laboratory (black curve). The dots in the right panel represent averages of performance lapses (reaction times longer than 500 ms) on a psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) [61] over 12 healthy adults. The whiskers represent the standard deviations over subjects, which reflect trait inter-individual differences [62] not further discussed in the present paper. Note how well the model predictions based on the dynamics of the homeostatic and circadian processes match the neurobehavioral performance averages. Figure taken from [63] with permission.

Many other factors besides the homeostatic and circadian processes affect neurobehavioral functioning. One noteworthy biological process additionally involved is sleep inertia, which produces the transient grogginess, neurobehavioral performance impairment and tendency to return to sleep experienced immediately after awakening [15]. As the sleep inertia effect dissipates rapidly within the first hour after awakening and is barely (if at all) still measurable after two hours [16, 17], it is not further discussed here.

Internal and external states can have a transient, albeit potentially powerful, effect on neurobehavioral performance. Performance may temporarily improve due to internal (e.g., motivation [18]) or external (e.g., bright light exposure [19]) factors. Conversely, it may temporarily deteriorate due to internal (e.g., anxiety [20]) or external (e.g., noise [21]) states as well. See Fig. (2) for a schematic of this interplay of multiple driving forces, in which the modifiers of neurobehavioral performance other than the homeostatic, circadian and sleep inertia processes are subsumed under the broad categories of endogenous and exogenous stimuli.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Schematic representation of the biological (homeostatic, circadian and sleep inertia) processes systematically driving temporal changes in waking neurobehavioral performance, and the transient modifying effects of endogenous (internal) and exogenous (external) stimuli. The circadian process is shown here as providing pressure for wakefulness that opposes the homeostatic pressure for sleep [57], which is one way to conceptualize the interplay between the two processes as they modulate neurobehavioral performance [3, 64]. A wide variety of internal and external influences may temporarily modify this interplay or otherwise affect neurobehavioral performance. Figure taken from [11] with permission.

In the present paper, the focus is on the main, continually operating neurobiological mechanisms of the homeostatic and circadian processes as they affect neurobehavioral performance. The circadian process is well characterized: the circadian rhythm is principally generated by the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) in the hypothalamus [22], and its effect on neurobehavioral function involves the locus ceruleus [23] and other subcortical structures [24]. The biobehavioral function and evolutionary advantage of the circadian process is also clearly recognized [25, 26]. However, the neurobiology of the homeostatic process and its effects on performance are poorly understood.

As discussed in other papers in this special issue, there is little evidence that the homeostatic process is fundamentally regulated centrally (top-down) and/or generated by specific brain structures; nor is it known precisely what its function or evolutionary significance is. Yet, the effects of the homeostatic process on waking neurobehavioral function can be described in detail, and this may shed some light on the underlying mechanisms. Before pursuing this topic, it is useful to discuss another facet of the temporal regulation of neurobehavioral function: the time-on-task effect.

TIME-ON-TASK EFFECT

The time-on-task effect constitutes a performance decrement building over the duration of a cognitive performance task, which seems to reflect a cumulative increase in the effort required to deploy cognitive resources. The phenomenon has been operationalized as a progressive decline in performance (longer reaction times and/or greater number of errors) the longer one is required to sustain attention to perform the task. It is particularly noticeable in vigilance performance tasks [27] and related tasks such as the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT; see [3]). Rest breaks (with or without sleep) and task switching provide recuperation from the time-on-task effect [2830].

Of relevance to our understanding of the mechanisms underlying neurobehavioral performance, the homeostatic and circadian processes interact with the time-on-task effect, such that the time-on-task effect is amplified during extended wakefulness and during the biological night [3133]; see Fig. (3). An unresolved question is whether this apparent interaction is additive or synergistic. In case of the latter, it could mean that extending time on task – which could be seen as increasing task-specific workload – results in a more rapid build-up of homeostatic pressure for sleep. This intriguing possibility would imply that the time-on-task effect and the homeostatic process have some common underlying neurobiology [34].

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Pattern of neurobehavioral performance across 36 hours of total sleep deprivation. In this laboratory study, the sleep deprivation period began at 10:00 hours, and performance was measured by mean reaction time on a 10-minute PVT administered every 2 hours (averages over 10 healthy young adults are shown). The curve displays the reaction time averages from test bout to test bout, tracking the combined effect of the homeostatic and circadian processes. The slanted lines show deterioration of performance – approximated with linear regression – during each 10-minute test bout, representing the time-on-task effect. The rate of deterioration was greater when overall neurobehavioral performance was more impaired, revealing an interaction between the time-on-task effect and the homeostatic and circadian processes. Note also that the rest breaks between the test bouts provided recuperation from the time-on-task effect: performance at the beginning of a test bout was typically better than that at the end of the previous test bout. Figure taken from [65] with permission.

The cause of the time-on-task effect in terms of brain function is not known. Using near-infrared optical imaging [35, 36], we measured hemodynamic signals during performance of a 10-minute PVT at multiple time points across 62 hours of total sleep deprivation [37]. We used this brain imaging technique, suitable for the recording of relative changes in brain activation across time on task, to gauge changes in regional brain metabolism as measured from the scalp over the prefrontal cortex in 12 healthy young adults (7 males, 5 females; ages 22–37). Bilateral prefrontal hemodynamic signals were measured with the Hamamatsu NIRO-200 near-infrared optical topography device every 2 hours during performance of the PVT, while head position was fixed with a chin rest. Prefrontal oxygenated hemoglobin (O2Hb) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) were sampled at 0.17 Hz across the task duration.

The 10-minute hemodynamic profiles thus acquired were averaged across the multiple PVT test bouts in an attempt to isolate brain metabolic correlates of the time-on-task effect independent of homeostatic and circadian influences – see Fig. (4). The results suggested an increasing but saturating profile for O2Hb over the 10 minutes of time on task, with an inverse profile for HHb, leaving total hemoglobin more or less constant. This finding might be interpreted as an increasing need for metabolic supply over time on task, which cannot be fully met and therefore leads to increased performance impairment as task duration progresses. However, a closer look at moment-to-moment task performance suggested that additional or altogether different mechanisms are at play, as described in the next section.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Left and right oxygenated (O2Hb-L, O2Hb-R), deoxygenated (HHb-L, HHb-R) and total (oxygenated + deoxygenated; THb-L, THb-R) hemoglobin measured over the scalp of the prefrontal cortex. Grand averages are shown (in relative, arbitrary units) as observed across repeated 10-minute PVT test bouts during 62 hours of total sleep deprivation in 12 healthy young adults.

WAKE STATE INSTABILITY

While sleep loss amplifies the time-on-task effect (see Fig. (3)), it also induces increased moment-to-moment performance variability [38, 39]. As such, the time-on-task effect is not strictly linear as the regression lines in Fig. (3) would suggest. Rather, it entails a progressive increase in response variability over the duration of the task [39, 40].

Fig. (5) shows the raw reaction time data of 10-minute PVT bouts performed by a single individual at 24-hour intervals across 60 hours of total sleep deprivation. The graphs show that performance deteriorated as time on task progressed, and that this occurred more rapidly the longer wakefulness was extended. However, the performance impairment did not take the form of a gradual lengthening of reaction times – it involved an increase in stochastic performance variability. In fact, on the PVT, performance impairment due to sleep loss results in what appears to be a mixture of normal task responses, false starts, and lapses of attention.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Raw reaction time (RT) data as observed during a 10-minute PVT for one individual undergoing total sleep deprivation. The panels show reaction times (on the ordinate) as a function of the successive stimuli (on the abscissa), after 12 hours, 36 hours and 60 hours of continuous wakefulness (each test bout took place at 20:00 hours). Gaps represent errors of commission (false starts). Figure taken from [41] as redrawn from [39] with permission.

Fig. (5) illustrates that both time on task and time awake induce stochastic performance variability. It has been posited that increased performance variability is a hallmark of the effect of the homeostatic process on neurobehavioral performance, and that this phenomenon is caused by wake state instability [39]. However, the similarity between the effect of time awake and that of time on task suggests that this theory needs to be refined, as it may need to account for the time-on-task effect and the homeostatic process having a shared underlying neurobiology.

It has been postulated that stochastic performance variability under conditions of sleep deprivation provides a key explanation for why sleep loss-related accidents tend to be rare but catastrophic [41]. This can be understood in the context of the Swiss cheese model of accident causation [42], which implies that accidents occur predominantly under circumstances when multiple potential vulnerabilities are aligned in time. For example, a sleep-deprived automobile driver would be poised to have an accident when he or she simultaneously experiences a) a lapse of attention when b) approaching an intersection while c) another car is already on the intersection. However, if the driver were sleep-deprived but did not experience a lapse of attention upon approaching the intersection, the accident would not occur. In this sense, the timing of lapses of attention constitutes a key mediator of the occurrence of sleep loss-related accidents, as illustrated in Fig. (6).

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

Schematic of a mechanism postulated to explain how sleep loss contributes to accidents. The top panel, copied from Fig. (5), depicts performance variability across a 10-minute interval at 60 hours of total sleep deprivation; extended bars represent lapses of attention. The middle panel shows a hypothetical pattern of changing cognitive demands over the 10-minute interval. The bottom panel displays the hypothetical impact of human error over the course of the 10-minute interval. Accidents are most likely to occur when lapses of attention line up temporally with high performance demands as well as substantial consequences of error, as where illustrated by the dotted gray line. Thus, accidents related to sleep loss are unusual (rare), but typically profound (catastrophic). The schematic can be adapted for specific operational settings by replacing the middle panel with one or more particular, temporally varying safety risk (e.g., for road transportation: traffic density, weather conditions, etc.). Figure adapted from [41] with permission.

The timing of lapses of attention is stochastic and thus unpredictable, and for moderate amounts of sleep loss, lapses are relatively infrequent (see Fig. (5), middle panel). This may help to explain why, say, a night shift worker may have the same drive home in the morning – day after day, year after year – without ever having an incident, until one day a lapse of attention temporally aligns with a dangerous traffic situation, and a collision ensues. In hindsight, it would be hard to reconstruct why, on that specific day, the person suddenly failed to drive safely. Had the pattern of his/her performance variability on the day of the accident and on earlier days been known, however, it might have been revealed that an accident was waiting to happen all along, whenever the timing of a lapse of attention were to coincide with a safety-critical event. In other words, performance variability from sleep loss does not necessarily cause an accident – but it does result in an increase in risk which may lead to an accident sooner or later [41].

In technologically advanced, safety-sensitive settings, ranging from automobiles, trains, planes and other modes of transportation to factories, oil platforms and nuclear reactors, many detection and warning systems help to promote and maintain safe operations. The role of human beings in these settings is often focused on monitoring the warning systems and taking action as soon as any operational safeguards break down. Optimal human responsiveness is critical then, so that back-up systems and safety procedures are employed in a timely manner to avert overall systems failure. It is under such circumstances that stochastic performance variability can be particularly devastating, as exemplified by a number of well-known nocturnal catastrophes (e.g., the Chernobyl nuclear disaster). In those instances, a better understanding of neurobehavioral performance impairment and the underlying mechanisms could make a crucial difference.

TASK DEPENDENCE OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION EFFECTS

The effects of sleep deprivation on neurobehavioral performance vary depending on the performance task considered [43, 44]. To some extent, this variation is due to confounds in performance measurements intrinsic to many tasks, such as practice effects and speed/accuracy trade-offs [11]. However, there is also variation in the effects of sleep deprivation depending on the specific cognitive processes involved in the tasks at hand [45], as sleep deprivation appears to affect distinct cognitive processes differentially [37].

The differential effects of sleep deprivation on distinct cognitive processes have been demonstrated recently with a modified Sternberg task [46] designed to distinguish working memory scanning efficiency and resistance to proactive interference from other aspects of cognition [47]. In this task, subjects were shown a set of either two or four consonant letters, which they were required to hold in working memory. They were then shown a probe letter, which was present in the memory set at 50% chance; and they were asked to determine, as quickly and accurately as possible, whether or not the probe was indeed in the memory set. The slope of the linear relationship between reaction times and memory set size (two or four), measured for correct responses across 128 test trials, was used to measure deficits in working memory scanning efficiency [48]. Further, half the probes in the task for which the correct response was “no” (i.e., negative probes) had been encountered in the preceding trial, which caused proactive interference. The difference between reaction times to the previously encountered (i.e., recent) negative probes and reaction times to the other (non-recent) negative probes was used to measure deficits in the ability to resist proactive interference [49].

In a laboratory, the modified Sternberg task was administered at baseline, 48 hours later after 51 hours of total sleep deprivation, and 48 hours later after two nights of recovery sleep [37]. As illustrated in Fig. (7), overall reaction times were significantly degraded during sleep deprivation as compared to baseline and following recovery. However, the slope of the linear relationship between reaction times and memory set size was not significantly altered by sleep deprivation Fig. (7), left panel), nor was the difference in reaction times between recent and non-recent negative probes Fig. (7), right panel). Thus, sleep deprivation did not significantly affect working memory scanning efficiency [37, 50] and resistance to proactive interference [37], respectively. The effects of sleep deprivation on overall reaction times were due to one or more of the other cognitive processes involved in performing the modified Sternberg task, such as probe encoding and/or response selection [37, 51].

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7

Effects of sleep deprivation on reaction times in a modified Sternberg task. Eleven healthy young adults performed the task at baseline (black dots), after 51 hours of total sleep deprivation (dark gray dots), and following two nights of recovery sleep (light gray dots). See the main text for details on the task. The left panel shows reaction times (mean ± standard error) for memory sets containing two items versus four items; the slopes of the lines represent working memory scanning efficiency. The right panel shows reaction times (mean ± standard error) for negative probes not seen recently versus negative probes seen recently (i.e., in the previous trial); the differences in reaction times represent the ability to resist proactive interference. Sleep deprivation caused an overall increase in reaction times regardless of set size or probe recency. However, working memory scanning efficiency and resistance to proactive interference were not significantly affected by sleep deprivation. Figure taken from [66]; data from [37].

BOTTOM-UP REGULATION OF TEMPORAL CHANGES IN NEUROBEHAVIORAL PERFORMANCE

How and why sleep deprivation affects distinct cognitive processes differentially is not well understood. The phenomenology suggests that sleep deprivation may affect brain function locally in those neuronal pathways that subserve the cognitive processes invoked during task performance. The existence of the time-on-task effect and its interaction with sleep loss further suggest a dependence on (cumulative) activation of the neuronal groups involved – i.e., use-dependence. Similar mechanisms are observed in sleep regulation, as recognized in the bottom-up theory of local, use-dependent sleep [52, 53] discussed in this special issue. Here we extend the theory to waking neurobehavioral performance – see Fig. (8).

Fig. 8.

Fig. 8

Simplified conceptual model of the effects of time on task, sleep deprivation and circadian rhythm on neurobehavioral performance. 1) On a time scale in the order of milliseconds, information processing in a neuronal assembly (group) associated with a given performance task triggers a biochemical cascade that induces the local sleep state. When the neuronal assembly is in the wake state and stimulated by input, it responds with synaptic transmission to process the input signal and generate corresponding output. This involves release of adenosine triphosphate (ATP) into the extracellular space and increased local metabolic activity. Breakdown of extracellular ATP results in accumulation of adenosine, at a rate approximately proportional to the amount of synaptic transmission in response to stimulation (i.e., use-dependent). Binding of adenosine at purine type 1 receptors promotes the neuronal assembly sleep state, during which the assembly’s synaptic transmission patterns are fundamentally altered. This functionally alters the contribution of the neuronal assembly to the coordinated response of the larger neuronal pathway involved in the performance task, causing degraded information processing. As such, the local sleep state causes output variability, which leads to (stochastic) neurobehavioral performance variability (see Fig. (5)). 2) On a time scale of minutes to hours, binding of ATP to purine type 2 receptors results in release of sleep regulatory substances (SRSs) such as tumor necrosis factor (TNF) and interleukin-1 (IL1). Continued use of the neuronal assembly causes these SRSs to accumulate and, via their receptors and nuclear factor κB (NF-κB, not shown), increase the density of post-synaptic adenosine (A1) receptors (purine type 1 receptors) [52]. As a consequence, the probability of entering the sleep state increases in a use-dependent manner, at the neurobehavioral level giving rise to the time-on-task effect. The SRSs also promote the neuronal assembly sleep state through activation of GABAergic inhibitory neurons. The GABAergic neurons inhibit the glutamatergic excitatory neurons, which prevents these glutamatergic neurons from promoting the local wake state. The SRSs together with metabolic products such as adenosine also influence regional blood supply (see Fig. (4)) and thereby oxygen and metabolic nutrient supply. A rest break allows SRS, ATP and adenosine levels to decay, resetting the time-on-task effect. 3) On a time scale of hours to days, basal metabolic activity present in all neuronal assemblies, and associated conversion of ATP to adenosine, leads to a build-up of SRSs over time awake. This is modulated by the SCN, which drive circadian rhythms in the cellular machinery and neuronal activation across the whole brain. Accordingly, the magnitude of the time-on-task effect is affected by both time awake and time of day (see Fig. (3)). Subcortical circuits involved in the coordination and consolidation of whole-brain sleep (depicted schematically in light gray) are influenced by the collective neuronal assembly states, integrated across the brain through neuronal mechanisms involving the SRSs. The subcortical circuits include the ventrolateral preoptic area (VLPO), which can shut down the wake-promoting (e.g., glutamatergic) neurons of the reticular activating system and other systems such as the cholinergic networks of the basal forebrain (not shown). These subcortical systems orchestrate sleep/wake states across the whole brain, and seek to induce global (i.e., whole-brain) sleep to prevent interaction with the environment when too many neuronal assemblies are in the local sleep state. The global sleep state allows SRS concentrations and receptor densities to be restored in a coordinated manner across all neuronal assemblies, resetting both the time-on-task effect and the time-awake effect in the process. Figure and caption adapted from [34] with permission.

We theorize that the regulation of temporal changes in neurobehavioral performance during sleep deprivation and across time-on-task, like the regulation of sleep, is fundamentally local and use-dependent in nature [34]. In particular, we hypothesize that neuronal groups involved in performing a given task will fall asleep locally as a homeostatic consequence of sustained use [54], thereby interrupting information processing and leading to performance impairment. This notion would explain a number of phenomena for which no satisfactory other explanation is available, as follows.

It should be emphasized that these explanations are based on theory derived from a mixture of scientific evidence regarding sleep regulation (expounded in this special issue) and regarding waking neurobehavioral performance (discussed in the present paper) as well as some reasoned speculation. Also, it is recognized that not all aspects of neurobehavioral performance impairment in the context of sleep loss can be explained in terms of local, use-dependent sleep. Notably, the circadian process is known to be centrally driven (see Fig. (8)), originating in the SCN [57] as discussed earlier. Certain correlates of sleep loss such as oculomotor changes [58] and slow eyelid closures [59] are also likely to be centrally mediated [60].

Nonetheless, these top-down, central (predominantly subcortical) mechanisms do not explain either the reason or the time course of the homeostatic process and its effects and interactions on waking neurobehavioral function. Our proposed explanations are effectively the same as those put forth for sleep regulation and sleep function in this special issue, deriving from the concept of local, use-dependent sleep. We posit that to understand the temporal regulation of neurobehavioral performance in sufficient detail, such a bottom-up perspective is essential.

Acknowledgments

We thank David Rector, Devon Grant, Adrienne Tucker, Scott Doran and David Dinges for their contributions to experimental results discussed here. Original findings presented in this paper were obtained in studies supported by the W.M. Keck Foundation, CDMRP award W81XWH-05-1-0099, and DURIP grant FA9550-06-1-0281.

References