The Rational Patient and Beyond: Implications for Treatment Adherence in People with Psychiatric Disabilities (original) (raw)
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Rehabil Psychol. 2014 Jan 20;59(1):85–98. doi: 10.1037/a0034935
Abstract
Purpose/Objective
Many people with psychiatric disabilities do not benefit from evidence-based practices because they often do not seek out or fully adhere to them. One way psychologists have made sense of this rehabilitation and health decision process and subsequent behaviors (of which adherence might be viewed as one) is by proposing a “rational patient;” namely, that decisions are made deliberatively by weighing perceived costs and benefits of intervention options. Social psychological research, however, suggests limitations to a rational patient theory that impact models of health decision making.
Design
The research literature was reviewed for studies of rational patient models and alternative theories with empirical support. Special focus was on models specifically related to decisions about rehabilitation strategies for psychiatric disability.
Results
Notions of the rational patient evolved out of several psychological models including the health belief model, protection motivation theory, and theory of planned behavior. A variety of practice strategies evolved to promote rational decision making. However, research also suggests limitations to rational deliberations of health. (1) Rather than carefully and consciously considered, many health decisions are implicit, potentially occurring outside awareness. (2) Decisions are not always planful; often it is the immediate exigencies of a context rather than an earlier balance of costs and benefits that has the greatest effects. (3) Cool cognitions often do not dictate the process; emotional factors have an important role in health decisions. Each of these limitations suggests additional practice strategies that facilitate a person’s health decisions.
Conclusions/Implications
Old models of rational decision making need to be supplanted by multi-process models that explain supra-deliberative factors in health decisions and behaviors.
Keywords: health decision making, implicit attitudes, cold cognitions
There is compelling evidence supporting several practices as state-of-the-art rehabilitation for people with psychiatric disabilities stemming from serious mental illnesses such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression (Corrigan, Mueser, Bond, Drake, & Solomon, 2008). In the past decade, coalitions of researchers supported by SAMHSA (Hennessy & Chambers, 2009), Johnson and Johnson (Drake, Becker, Goldman, & Martinez, 2006) and NIMH (Dixon et al., 2010) have identified several evidence-based practices (EBPs) including medication management, assertive community treatment, supported employment, permanent supportive housing, family education and support, illness management and recovery, and integrated treatment for co-occurring disorders. Unfortunately, many people with psychiatric disabilities who might benefit from these practices do not seek them out when in need. Many who do begin treatment decide to end it prematurely.
One way behavioral scientists have made sense of poor treatment adherence is positing people as rational actors making carefully considered decisions about health care options and wellness. In this light, “rational patients” are viewed as coolly and solely weighing costs and benefits in a prior and planful manner. Counseling strategies including shared decision making and motivational interviewing help the person with the deliberative process. However, there may be limits to the model; we propose and review three supra-deliberative processes in this paper. (1) Although it seems that rational patients make “conscious” comparisons of the advantages and disadvantages of treatment decisions, many real-life decisions are made implicitly and out of awareness. (2) Rather than planning with foresight, rehabilitation and treatment decisions are often influenced by immediate exigencies. It is not the consideration on Monday that leads to whether the person takes medication Thursday afternoon, but more likely salient issues from Thursday morning. (3) The idea of the rational patient suggests cold and calculated decisions when in fact many of these are emotional enterprises. We review the supra-deliberative factors herein by summarizing evidence that expands the narrow assertion about the rational patient into more complex behavior representing the multifaceted experience of adherence. The review includes examination of strategies shown to enhance health decision making in these different ways.
Critiques of rational patient models are neither new nor innovative in terms of human behavior in general or health behavior per se (Bickel & Vuchinich, 2000). Sociologists, economists, and game theorists have explained help seeking behavior beyond a simple rational process. Still, the idea of the rational patient remains strong. For example, the Adherence Research Network, begun in 2010 by the National Institutes of Health, lists rational models most prominently among its strategies to influence treatment decisions. Hence, we do not believe rational patient models have been dismissed at least not from the real-word practice setting. We begin the paper by briefly reviewing the literature on treatment adherence and mental health disabilities, in the process highlighting both the complexity of and problems with the construct. We then summarize rational actor models that describe these kinds of decisions and segue to a review of strategies meant to enhance deliberation related to treatment and rehabilitation.
We contrast these models with behavioral economic and other critiques that have been extended to adherence. We organize the next section of the paper in terms of limitations to rational patient models; this section includes tentative proposals for how adherence might be augmented should these limitations be further supported by research. We end by considering what these alternatives mean for ongoing study of treatment adherence as well as other health decisions and behaviors. We use the term patient with significant hesitation in the paper acknowledging the benefit of person first language in the rehabilitation literature. We chose to stick with “rational patient” at places because it reflects the relatively unidimensional perspectives of the medical model from which deliberative perspectives on adherence largely emerged.
Understanding Treatment Adherence
It is almost an axiom of health services and sciences that many people do not adhere to treatment. The World Health Organization (2003), Institute of Medicine (2008), and U.S. Surgeon General (1999) have all identified treatment adherence among topmost priorities in the real world use of evidence-based therapies and rehabilitation. Operational definitions of adherence differentiate care seeking (i.e., Does a person seek out a physician or other health care provider when symptoms and disabilities become distressing and the person is in need of services?) from service participation (Does the person remain actively engaged in treatment once it begins?). Poor treatment adherence is ubiquitous across medical conditions and disabilities: cancer (Partridge, Wang, Winer, & Avorn, 2003); heart, lung, and blood disease (Fung, Huang, Brand, Newhouse, & Hsu, 2007); childhood conditions (Celano et al., 2010); neurological and psychiatric illnesses (Gilmer et al., 2004); alcohol and substance abuse (Terra et al., 2006); and allergy and infectious diseases (Remien et al., 2005). Although adherence typically refers to peoples taking medications as prescribed, researchers now recognize it impacts most other clinical disciplines and related treatments (Shumaker, Ockene, & Riekert, 2009). Many recipients of surgical interventions, for example, ignore pre- and post-op tests and procedures (Stephens et al., 2009). People with orthopedic disabilities forget physical rehabilitation (Brewer et al., 2004).
The problem in treatments for people with serious mental illnesses seems especially bleak. Researchers have summarized the issue well elsewhere (Kreyenbuhl, Nossel, & Dixon, 2009; Velligan et al., 2009, 2010a); interesting findings from there are summarized in Table 1. Prominent among these is research from the Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study (Narrow, Regier, Rae, & Manderscheid, 1993), the National Comorbidity Survey (Kessler et al., 2001), and the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (Kessler & Merikangas, 2004) that show 30 to 40% of people who might benefit from psychotropic medication fail to seek it out. Findings from literature reviews and national surveys by the Schizophrenia Patient Outcome Research Team examined participation in psychiatric rehabilitation services (Dixon et al., 2010; Lehman et al., 1998). Although more than 90% of individuals in the survey reported receiving maintenance neuroleptic treatment, less than half acknowledged participation in any psychosocial services, less than a quarter were involved in family therapy, and only about 10% received some variation of assertive community treatment. Dropout is an important indicator of nonadherence. Recent findings from the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) schizophrenia study showed that 74% of research participants dropped out of a medication trial during the 18 months of the study (Lieberman et al., 2005; Stroup & Appelbaum, 2006). Another index of service participation is failure to attend outpatient appointments. McCarthy et al (2007) showed as many as 25% of veterans with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder had one or more gaps in contact. Data from the NCS-Replication showed a similar overall 25% dropout rate from mental health treatment with significantly worse rates for those with marked disabilities (Olfson et al., 2009).
Table 1.
Rates of failing to adhere to different treatments for various illnesses.
ILLNESS | TREATMENT OR PROCEDURE | FAILURE TO ADHERE | CITATIONS |
---|---|---|---|
MENTAL ILLNESS | |||
Schizophrenia | medications | 50–74% | Gilmer et al., 2004; Kessler, et al., 2001; Jones et al, 1999; Lehman et al., 1998; Lieberman et al., 2005; Narrow et al., 1993; Smith, 2006; Stroup & Appelbaum, 2006 |
Schizophrenia | psychosocial treatments | ~50% | Dixon et al., 2010; Lehman et al., 1998 |
Bipolar disorder | medication or psychosocial treatments | 25% | McCarthy et al., 2007 |
General mental Illness | psychosocial treatments | 22.4% | Olfson et al., 2009 |
Co-occurring mental illness and substance abuse | 12 step programs | 75–85% | Terra et al., 2006 |
OTHER ILLNESSES OR DISABILITIES | |||
Cancer | Medication | 50% | Partridge et al., 2003 |
Heart, Lung, & Blood | Medication | 12–28% | Fung et al., 2007 |
Childhood Asthma | MedicationDoctors Visits | 43%60% | Celano et al., 2010 |
Despite these adherence rates, failing to fully engage in treatment is not a problem of mental illness alone. The bottom half of Table 1 includes adherence to medication rates for some physical illnesses. Up to 60% of those struggling with illnesses related to cancer, heart disease, and childhood asthma did not comply with medication prescriptions or doctor’s visits.
At the most obvious, poor adherence worsens morbidity and mortality which directly harms people with psychiatric disabilities. But its effects reach far beyond symptoms and illness course. Poor adherence exacerbates psychiatric disabilities undermining family relationships, work, and other social responsibilities including parenting and wage earning. Nonadherence taxes limited health resources that could be used better elsewhere. It stymies innovation. It adds to health provider burnout. For some conditions, it may imperil the public including increased crime and unnecessary accidents.
One way to expand discussion on adherence is to recognize it as one example of the broader set of decisions and behaviors related to a person’s pursuit of health and wellness. Adherence is both a decision (Yes, I will meet with my job coach today.) and a behavior (The person and job coach actually come together at a prospective job site.). Hence, research and theory should focus not only on whether the person is doing what he or she should, but also on processes that affect independent decision making and behaviors related to health. This broadens the discussion to include insights from psychological models related to decision making which we use in the next section to make sense of the hypothesized rational patient.
Models of Rational Patients
Several theories have developed to explain adherence as a rational process -- the person making health decisions based on the balance of perceived costs and benefits; these are briefly summarized in Table 2. More than 50 years ago, the health belief model (HBM) was posed by health care providers and services researchers as a decisional process influenced by weighting and contrasting the harm of a clinical condition and the benefits of available treatments (Champion & Skinner, 2008; Rosenstock, 1966, 1974). Clinical harm was defined as perceived susceptibility (the likelihood that an individual will contract a condition) and perceived severity (the impact of a condition as measured by symptoms, quality of life, and disability). Although HBM was developed long ago and has attracted careful and comprehensive critiques since (Stroebe & Strobe, 1995), it continues to be incorporated into contemporary research in health psychology because it is an effective heuristic for research. Closely related to HBM is protection motivation theory (PMT, Armitage & Conner, 2000; Rogers, 1983). In PMT, health behavior is framed as adaptive or maladaptive coping which is determined by protection motivation. This kind of motivation is influenced by threat appraisals (like HBM, based on perceived vulnerability, severity of illness, and depth of disability), and coping appraisals which interjects social learning constructs such as self-efficacy. Meta-analyses completed on HBM (Harrison, Mullen, & Green, 1992) and PMT (Milne, Sheehan, & Orbell, 2000) yield small to medium effect sizes in their explanation of health decisions.
Table 2.
Models of the rational patient contrasted with supra-deliberative processes.
RATIONAL PATIENT | SUPRA-DELIBERATIVE PROCESSES |
---|---|
Health belief model: health decisions specific to individual diseases are influenced by perceptions of susceptibility to the disease, disease severity, barriers to intervention, and treatment benefits. | Implicit decision making: decisions are made outside of awareness, relatively effortlessly, and quickly. |
Protection motivation theory: health decisions are effected by the appraised: threats of a condition and resources to do with the threat. | Immediate decision making: rather than planful, the immediate characteristics of a situation often drive a decision. |
Self-efficacy: confidence in addressing health conditions is an important moderator in understanding health decisions. | Emotional decision making: affective characteristics of a situation impact its analysis and subsequent decisions. |
Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA): in addition to perceived threats and consequences of a health condition, social norms, the perception of important others, are impactful moderators. | |
Theory of Planned Behavior: this extends TRA with notions of perceived control. |
Research on human decision making in general is also relevant. According to Bandura’s (1977) social learning theory, it is not the assortment of past reinforcing and punishing consequences of a health behavior that influence adherence but rather how past learning has been experienced and the expectations those experiences engender. Bandura (1986) further elaborated on social learning effects with the idea of self-efficacy. Self-efficacy represents confidence in one’s ability to carry out specific behaviors in specific settings. The Theory of Reasoned Action seems to be a more direct social learning perspective relevant to understanding health decisions and behaviors (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). Like previous theories, the Theory of Reasoned Action proffers positive and negative evaluation of a behavior as central to moving ahead on said behavior. The Theory also suggests perception of social norms as an equally important element of reasoned action; namely, recognition of an important other’s preference vis-à-vis health behavior moderates the person’s decisions. Ajzen (1988) conceded that the Theory of Reasoned Action was limited to volitional behavior and developed the Theory of Planned Behavior by adding notions of perceived control. Perceived control represents actual management in a specific setting and a measure of confidence in one’s ability in that setting (similar to self-efficacy). Findings from a meta-analysis of 96 studies of the Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior specifically for adherence yielded moderate to high effect sizes (Albarracin, Johnson, Fishbein, & Muellerleile, 2001).
Critiques of Rational Patient Models
Sociologists have developed several theories that explain help seeking behavior beyond rational processes (Friedman & Hechter, 1988; Lindenberg, 1985). Pescosolido (1992) outlined helping behavior via social networks and help seeking episodes. Accordingly, it is not the solo, planful consideration of contingencies that drive adherence but rather the dynamic, often immediate, and sometimes “irrational” products of social networks from which health decisions and behaviors evolve (Smith & Christakis, 2008). Theories and research from behavioral economists have provided more specific, cognitive analyses of the rational patient (Luce, 2000; Simon, 1987). They have argued that people often rely on cognitive shortcuts which may not be the most “rational” but serve the perceived needs of the moment (Volpp, Friedman, Romano, Rosen, & Silber, 2010). For example, research by Kahneman and Tversky (1979) on cognitive heuristics shows decisions and corresponding behaviors often rest on “approximate” rules of thumb rather than strict logic. The availability heuristic -- the notion that “truth” reflects the simple exposure to a phenomenon (Tversky & Kahneman, 1973) -- offers a compelling example: a person believes cigarette smoking is not health-threatening because “my father smoked two packs a day for 50 years.” Although these models have not been as widely applied to health decisions (and even less so to questions of adherence), they are an important and growing research paradigm.
How to Influence the Rational Patient
In some ways, then, critiques of the rational patient model might seem like old news; that understanding adherence from the perspective of HBM and TPB is dated. And perhaps this is true from the theoretical perspective of basic behavioral scientists. But a different picture emerges when considering research regarding how to influence services and health decisions. Findings from systematic reviews and meta-analyses looking at impacting adherence related to HIV-AIDS (Simoni, Pearson, Pantalone, Marks, & Crepaz, 2006), childhood illnesses (Graves, Roberts, Rapoff, & Boyer, 2010), disorders in older adults (Conn et al., 2009), and psychiatric disability (Gaudiano, Weinstock, & Miller, 2008; Velligan et al., 2010) showed interventions grouping into two clusters: those meant to influence deliberation (the rational patient per se) and those meant to augment deliberation such as psychoeducation, time management, provider communication skills, medication monitoring, and cognitive remediation. These are the practical strategies for augmenting deliberative processes related to health decision making. Two of these are described here: shared decision making and motivational interviewing.
Shared decision making (SDM) was developed to address treatment adherence by public health and medical providers (Edwards, Davies, & Edwards, 2009; Joosten et al., 2008). It frames health decisions as basically an exchange between person and health provider with three components. (1) Heeding the rational patient, the dyad attempts to identify advantages and disadvantages of a specific health behavior. (2) This often requires education about the illness and corresponding treatments. Various channels have been used to provide this information with research on internet applications and social networks being especially promising in the past couple of years (Tanis, 2008). (3) SDM is fundamentally social exchange so counseling skills that enhance engagement are important. These include active listening skills as well as person-centered notions of genuineness, empathy, and unconditional positive regard (Rudnick & Roe, 2010). Research has examined SDM for a variety of illnesses and disabilities including treatment decisions related to cancer (Gattellari, Butow, & Tattersall, 2001; van Roosmalen et al., 2004), prostatic hypertrophy (Murray et al., 2001), substance abuse (Joosten et al., 2008), GI disorders (Greenfield, Kaplan, & Ware, 1985) and mental illness (Ludman et al., 2003; Malm, Ivarsson, Allebeck, Falloon, 2003). Meta-analyses and reviews of studies yielded mixed conclusions (Bauer, 2002; Joosten et al., 2009; Nosé, Barbui, & Tansella, 2003). Results showed SDM leads to enhanced satisfaction with and more knowledge about treatments and their providers. Adherence seemed to improve in about one quarter to as many as two thirds, of research participants. But improvement in adherence frequently diminished in the weeks after intervention ceased. Moreover, characteristics of research participants, especially as related to their rational capabilities, seemed to diminish effects.
Motivational interviewing might be viewed as a form of SDM, originally developed as a treatment for substance abuse and extended as an adjunct for services in other illnesses (Martins & McNeil, 2009; Mitcheson, 2009) including psychiatric disability (McCracken & Corrigan, 2008). As such, it is an often used as a practical strategy for promoting rational patients. Motivational interviewing presumes stages of change that are defined by the relationship between perceived costs and benefits of a health decision (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1982, 1986). Stages of change theory is significant because it identifies people who are not ready to participate in treatment because they do not believe the clinical condition or disability is dire or in need of response. Using job goals as an example, people in what have been called the contemplation stage might admit to some disadvantages in not working (limited income, decreased social network, missed sense of industry) but believe its actual costs are minimal (this is better than risking a psychotic relapse). Precontemplation is the stage where the person fails to recognize any costs to the condition (returning to work is not really a priority of mine. After all, my roommate does not work.).
Motivational interviewing (MI) is a clinical intervention meant to impact contemplation and precontemplation stages of change. It expands the simple cost-benefit balance sheet into a counseling process resting on four basic principles: express empathy, develop discrepancy, roll with resistance, and support self-efficacy (Miller & Rollnick, 2002). Results of a meta-analysis (DiMatteo et al., 2002) as well as from individual studies of MI and treatment adherence have yielded positive findings for HIV-AIDS (Diiorio et al., 2008), smoking cessation (Thyrian et al., 2007), asthma (Schmaling, Blume, & Afari, 2001), substance abuse (Smith, Hall, Jang, & Arndt, 2009) and, specifically germane to the goals of this paper, the disabilities related to schizophrenia (Drymalski & Campbell, 2009). These studies also showed MI to be limited in its impact on heath decision making.
Perhaps one reason SDM and MI effects are limited for people with serious psychiatric disabilities is the impact of cognitive deficits; as a result of information processing dysfunctions, some people are unable to understand the balance of advantages and disadvantages of specific health decisions. People with schizophrenia show deficits in attention, memory, and executive functioning that may undermine adherence decisions (Kim et al., 2006). People in the acute stage of illness often experience psychotic symptoms that may skew decision making and medication adherence (Alvarez-Jimenez et al., 2009). Many people with illnesses in the schizophrenia spectrum show lower intelligence that may be due to the illness itself or its chronic course (Xiang, Shum, Chiu, Tang, & Ungvari, 2010). Regardless, some intellectual deficits have been shown to be associated with poor decision making related to medication management (Willard, 2006). Many people with schizophrenia lack insight into their illness (Buckley et al., 2007). Absence of disease awareness is associated with poor adherence (Beck, Cavelti, Kvrgic, Kleim & Vauth, 2011). Further complicating this picture is the distinct impact of diminished cognitive processing, and its effect on rational decision making, from cognitive disorganization which might diminish follow through on recommendations.
Towards this end, cognitive therapies have been developed and tested to enhance a person’s treatment decisions and behaviors. Although some of these therapies have targeted thought contents (e.g., inaccurate beliefs about the harm of a medication) in the Beckian approach to cognitive behavior therapy -- i.e., challenging irrational thoughts that undermine some health decisions (Wright, Kingdon, Turkington et al., 2008) -- more successful has been cognitive rehabilitation attempting to diminish the processing deficits that undermine comprehension of these decisions. Cognitive adaptation training (CAT) is a good example of the latter with growing supportive evidence (Velligan et al., 2002; Velligan et al., 2008). One way cognitive rehabilitation addresses information processing deficits is by providing tasks that “exercise” key processes such as attention and memory through many repetitions of computer tasks (Wykes & Reeder, 2005). CAT is an example of the alternative, what seems to be a more effective approach to cognitive rehabilitation; namely, the provision of compensatory strategies and environmental supports specifically built around real-world tasks of the individual. In order to help people with cognitive difficulties that result from some psychiatric disabilities, they might work with a rehabilitation counselor to clear out old prescriptions and over-the-counter medications from the bathroom cabinet; set all current prescriptions in an organized, daily pill box; and record a message in the person’s own voice tied to a computer alarm that reminds them, “It’s time to take my medication.”
Despite problems posed by cognitive deficits, we need to make sure misunderstandings and stigma about psychiatric disabilities do not derail the argument of our paper (Corrigan, 2005). Not everyone with serious mental illness is handicapped by cognitive dysfunctions. Those with cognitive dysfunctions are not necessarily unable to make decisions about their treatment and health. Cognitive deficits vary with some periods of remission being common, periods when decision making capacities are fine. Most people with psychiatric disabilities are usually able to participate in MI and SDM. Hence, the rational patient model is relevant for understanding treatment decisions and behaviors in people with psychiatric disabilities. And hence, limitations of those models are equally relevant. Three limitations are summarized earlier in Table 2 and more full developed here.
1. Many Decisions About Health are Implicit
Many treatment decisions are made outside awareness. Individuals may automatically associate medication or other treatment with disadvantages, irrespective of whether they deliberately endorse the proposition that the therapy is good. Social psychological research has convincingly shown that implicit, automatically activated associations can differ from deliberately endorsed propositions (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; Greenwald & Nosek, 2009). Implicit reactions are typically characterized by rapidity, spontaneity (lack of conscious intention), efficiency (minimal use of attentional resources), inevitability (lack of control over initial activation), and unawareness of triggering cues (Bargh, 1994; Gawronski & Bodenhausen 2006). In this context, implicitness suggests the “concept” of rehabilitation or health behavior can automatically activate positive or negative attributes if these concepts are associated with one another in memory. Well-established reaction-time tasks such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT: Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz 1998) measure automatic associations between two concepts (e.g., Medication’ and ‘Good’), and thus may index implicit attitudes toward treatments. Because indirect measures of implicit reactions are less susceptible to strategic response distortions and self-presentational strategies, they are particularly helpful in work on health decisions where participants may hesitate to openly endorse negative attitudes to treatments. In addition, implicit versus explicit responses often independently predict outcome variables. A recent meta-analysis on the IAT concluded that implicit reactions typically predicted behavior beyond that explained by explicit reactions (Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009).
Consistent with research on the IAT, implicit processes have been likened to simple associations between various products of information processing (frequently memory of attitudes or stereotypes) (Greenwald et al., 2009). For example, implicit positive attitudes about Drug X at moment Y are associated with a subsequent decision to take medication at that moment. Alternatively, implicit processes are described as a wholly functioning executive that occur outside awareness, seemingly transforming information to meet the demands of the situation; Dijksterhuis (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2010; Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006) calls this incubation or the “sleep on it” effect. A person, for example, may spontaneously seem to understand costs and benefits of a decision about a health behavior where he or she has been stumped in the past. “Now I see why I should work with a job coach to find employment.”
Although not well-developed for health decisions, some research has found associations between implicit attitudes and health decisions and behaviors including food choice and consumption (Friese, Hofmann, & Wänke, 2008), adolescents’ impulsive school behavior (Navarick, 2004), condom use (Broaddus, 2009) and marijuana consumption (Scott-Sheldon, 2006). There is also a fairly extensive body of research that has examined implicit reactions related to addictions. Implicit positive reactions to alcohol significantly predicted drinking in adolescents (Thush & Wiers, 2007) and stronger implicit associations of alcohol with positive arousal were related to increased alcohol use (Houben, Rothermund, & Wiers, 2009). Negative implicit attitudes toward smoking were associated with abstinence independent of explicit attitudes (Kahler et al., 2007) and predicted lower levels of craving and tobacco dependence (Waters et al., 2007). Finally, a single study by our group examined explicit versus implicit attitudes to psychiatric medication among 85 people with schizophrenia and similar psychiatric disabilities (Rüsch, Todd, Bodenhausen, Weiden, & Corrigan, 2009). Explicit, but not implicit, positive attitudes to psychiatric medication predicted higher self-reported medication adherence. However, implicit, not explicit, positive attitudes to medication predicted stronger insight into having a mental illness and stronger perceived need for treatment. Of note, implicit and explicit attitudes were not correlated, suggesting that the two represent distinct constructs. This study indicated that implicit attitudes to treatment can be measured and explain variance of treatment-related variables independent of explicit attitudes. Implicit attitudes’ effect on treatment-related cognitions and behaviors need to continue to be examined in future research.
Influencing Implicit Processes
There are practice-oriented clinical approaches (POCAs) that seem to improve largely automated, everyday health behaviors (Sritharan & Gawronski, 2010). An intervention among older adults to impact blood glucose monitoring successfully used detailed implementation plans including imagination techniques in order to enhance adherence during largely automated daily routines of participants (Liu & Park, 2004). Specifically, research participants were instructed to imagine themselves at home completing a glucometer task in a step-by-step fashion. Participants in this group performed nearly 50% more daily screening tasks than those in comparison conditions. In a laboratory-based setting, Wiers and colleagues (2010) used an evaluative conditioning procedure -- the repeated pairing of alcohol-related and negative stimuli -- to modify implicit attitudes related to alcohol abuse. It is thought that pairing alcohol-related stimuli with words or pictures of negative valence leads to new affective associations with the attitude object. Evaluative conditioning was shown to induce more negative implicit attitudes, less alcohol consumption, and less craving (Houben, Havermans, & Wiers, 2010; Houben, Schoenmakers, & Wiers, 2010). Another study involved approach- and avoidance-reactions with a computer joystick to alcohol-related stimuli; participants learned avoidance- and unlearned approach-behaviors which in turn predicted subsequent alcohol consumption (Wiers, Rinck, Kordts, Houben, & Strack, 2010). Although implicit reactions may be improved by evaluative conditioning, investigators caution that such interventions are only effective when also targeting explicit attitudes (Sritharan & Gawronski 2010). The same is likely to apply to interventions meant to impact health and rehabilitation decisions.
2. Immediate Processes Influence Health Decisions and Behaviors
The idea of rational health decision making sometimes suggests health and treatment are discrete phenomena with clear beginning and endpoints. This seems to frame treatment participation solely as an outcome which misguides some researchers into one time end measurements. Pescosolido (1992) redefined finite health decision and beliefs as more continuous experience and dynamics. Viewing treatment decisions and behaviors as one of many experiences on a healthcare trajectory frames the experience in terms of the stream of social life. Assessment of single and relatively “distant” health decisions fails to capture the phenomenon of actually doing treatment and rehabilitation. Experiences are dynamic and influenced by ever changing social forces. These are external and internal mediators that impact relationships between health decisions addressing similar needs at different times. The Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior further expand on the immediacy of decisions and behaviors (Ajzen, 1988; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980); the paradigms do not explain behaviors per se but behavioral intentions. Behavioral intentions are most likely to reflect actual behavior when the intention occurs relatively soon before the setting and time in which the behavior was meant to happen.
In terms of health behavior, research has shown that recollections of painful/unpleasant medical procedures are strongly influenced by the most recent or “end” characteristics of the procedure (Redelmeier & Kahneman, 1996; Redelmeier, Katz, & Kahneman, 2003). Ironically, people seem to favor longer procedures if they entail less discomfort at the end of the procedure, even if earlier steps produced notable pain intensity (Kahneman, Fredrickson, Schrieber, & Redelmeier, 1993; Redelmeier, Katz, & Kahneman, 2003). The significance of momentary characteristics of a procedure impacting adherence behaviors is often overlooked in existing research which examined predictors of poor adherence: long-term, fixed variables such as cognitive characteristics, gender, age, socioeconomic status, and type of illness (DiMatteo, 2004; DiMatteo, Giordani, Lepper, H& Croghan, 2002). Although the study of stable variables may be useful for determining systemic resource allocation and preemptive/prevention efforts in populations at risk for adherence problems, prescribed treatments most often require ongoing daily effort which are facilitated or hindered by one’s immediate context. Adherence is not an isolated decision with a predetermined aftermath. Rather, adherence is a process that occurs in ever-changing intrapersonal, interpersonal, and physical conditions that may impact one’s ability or motivation to engage in, or curtail, daily behaviors.
Influencing the Immediate Context
Correlational or retrospective designs are incapable of capturing the dynamic nature of health decisions and behaviors, or of identifying the role immediate/contextual factors may play. Correlational models, for example, provide “snapshots” of one’s adherence and context, but are ill-equipped to examine fluctuating behavior or contextual change. A retrospective view will encompass periods in which dynamic processes likely took place, but rely upon research participants’ ability to recall and summarize their experiences accurately, a process shown to produce biased reports when compared to real-time ratings (Ben-Zeev & Young, 2010; Ben-Zeev, Young, & Madsen, 2009, Ebner-Priemer et al., 2006). Experience Sampling Methods (ESM) offer a measurement model that is ideally-suited to capture the ebb-and-flow of adherence behaviors and examine the possible effects of immediate contextual factors (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987). ESM (also known as ambulatory assessment or ecological momentary assessment) is an intensive, repeated measures strategy that utilizes an electronic device to prompt participants to complete self-report questionnaires about their immediate thoughts, feelings, bodily sensations, activities, and settings, in real-time, multiple times a day. Data collected in ESM studies reflect an individual’s immediate or most recent state within the context and flow of daily experience, facilitating the study of microprocesses that influence behavior in real-world contexts (Shiffman, Stone, & Hufford, 2008). With real-time data capture, researchers can map the temporal sequence or co-occurrence of events over a given period of data collection, identifying immediate and/or short-term relationships between variables within individuals (Ebner-Priemer, Eid, Kleindienst, Stabenow, & Trull, 2009). Recent studies using this approach have produced some insights into relationships between immediate contextual factors and health related behaviors including cigarette smoking in individuals attempting to quit (Shiffman & Kirchner, 2009), physical activity in older adults (Dunton, Atienza, Castro, & King, 2009), self-injurious behaviors in adolescents (Nock, Prinstein, & Sterba, 2009), and self-medication of symptoms via substance use in schizophrenia (Swendsen, Ben-Zeev, & Granholm, 2010).
Pioneering research has explored the utility of mobile technologies for Ecological Momentary Interventions (EMI) that provide real-time, real-world support for a variety of health decisions (Depp et al., 2010; Heron & Smyth, 2010). For example, providers might equip people with personal digital assistants which provide repeated, real time reminders of previously agreed upon health decisions. Time, place, and content of reminders are set as an algorithm based on previous ESM findings. One study showed this kind of technology enhanced medication adherence in 42 people with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder who completed the task (Granholm et al., in press). EMI can be further enhanced with interactive voice response systems integrated into platforms meant to enhance quality and diversity of cues (Freedman, Lester, McNamara, Milby, & Schumacher, 2006; Levin & Levin, 2006).
3. Many Health Decisions are Influenced by Emotions
Emotions influence health decision making independent of cognition both directly and indirectly which seemingly challenges premises of the rational patient. First, emotions have been likened to motivation with people making specific health decisions in order to obtain or avoid consequences of emotions that accompany those decisions. In this way, motivation activates the decision making task. Second, research has found the interoceptive sequelae of emotions may have direct, not cognitively mediated, effects on adherence. In part, this is an arousal function, a more nonspecific version of psychological activation. Each of these implications is considered more fully in turn.
Emotions motivate health decisions
Kahneman (1999) proposed Hedonic Theory to argue that perceived utility of a decision or behavior is influenced by emotional response to the experience which subsequently impacts whether said decision or behavior will be repeated. Consistent with this Theory, research has shown emotions correspond with a range of health decisions with some (negatively valenced) seeming to undermine health decisions while others (more positively charged) enhancing decisions (Magai, Consedine, Neugut, & Hershman, 2007). Hedonic Theory has been used to understand emotion’s effects on health conditions and behavior including obesity (Cota, Tschop, Horvath, & Levine, 2006; Mela, 2006), smoking (Blendy et al., 2005; Dawkins, Acaster, & Powell, 2007), substance abuse (Berridge, 2007; Stevens, Peschk, & Schwarz, 2007), and exercise (Williams, 2008).
Research has fairly consistently shown depression undermines adherence to many treatment regimens with one meta-analysis yielding an odds ratio greater than three (DiMatteo, Lepper, & Croghan, 2000). People who are depressed are less likely to adhere to hypertensive (Lannin, Matthews, Mitchell, Swanson, & Edwards, 1998) and HIV (van Servellen, Chang, Garcia, & Lombardi, 2002) medication prescriptions as well as renal dialysis (Brownbridge & Fielding, 1994), mammography or other procedures for breast cancer (Magai, Consdeine, Neugut, & Hershman, 2007), and oral hypoglycemic treatments for diabetes (Kalsekar et al., 2006). Research on the effects of anxiety, which would seem to be similarly valenced, is mixed showing the complexity of linking adherence to quality of negative affect. Consider adherence to interventions related to breast cancer (Magai et al. 2007). There is a significant relationship between cancer fear and screening but, interestingly, a significant and inverse relationship between fear related to hospitalization, surgery, or mammography screening (Consedine, Magai, Krivoshekova, Ryzewicz, & Neugutl, 2004). People highly “emotional” and concerned about pain are less likely to adhere to pharmacological treatment for chronic pain (Nicklas, Dunbar, & Wild, 2010). A particularly social aspect of anxiety -- embarrassment -- is also related to adherence (Rüsch, Todd, Bodenhausen, Olschewski, & Corrigan, 2010). People will avoid illness screening or other aspects of treatment in order to escape the stigma of that illness (Corrigan, 2004; Garbers, Jessop, Foti, Uribelarrea, & Chiasson, 2003).
Interoception and arousal
Interoception might be simply defined as sensitivity to stimuli originating within the body, especially relevant to emotion vis-à-vis heart rate, muscle tension, and skin conductance. It has also broadly been equated to visceral or somatic markers and associated with constructs related to physiological arousal and activation. Significant research has examined the relationship between proxies of interoception and emotional arousal. Studies have shown better heart beat detection translates into greater intensity of self-reported affect (Critchey et al., 2004; Wiens et al., 2000). More relevant to the point here, research examining the relationship between interoception, arousal, and decision making has shown visceral effects of emotion partially influence decision making via its impact on perceived reinforcement and punishment of outcomes of that decision (Damasio, 1994; Naqvi & Bechara, 2009). These kinds of processes specifically influence health-related decision making (Hall, Ekkekakis, & Petruzzello, 2010; Jollant, Lawrence et al., 2010). Important here is the recognition that many of these processes occur outside awareness consistent with the reflexive nature of emotions (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997). In fact, research has identified a slew of psychological processes relevant to health decision making that are emotionally driven outside of awareness including appraisal of: threat (Adolphs et al., 2005), others’ traits (Willis & Todorov, 2006; Winston, Strange, O’Doherty, & Dolan, 2002), and social rewards (Cloutier, Heatherton, Whalen, & Kelley, 2008).
The Impact of Non-Cognitive Behavior Therapies
One of the goals of many cognitive therapies is to teach people how to manage out of control emotions by reframing the appraisals that correspond with them. In this light, cognitive therapy seeks to help the rational patient. Alternatively, knowledge that emotions are independent psychological processes, often affecting human behavior and experience separate from cognition, has significantly influenced relatively recent and innovative perspectives about change strategies. Two principles have emerged to expand the potential of CBT: acceptance and mindfulness. Acceptance suggests hurtful psychiatric problems and symptoms can be overcome, not by fighting symptoms per se, but by recognizing those symptoms and accepting them as an authentic part of the person’s condition at that moment (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999). Hence, contrary to many CBT approaches which would suggest negative emotional reactions to health should somehow be controlled, notions of acceptance encourage the person to recognize the positive or negatively valenced affect in response to the health condition or treatment, and use these emotional reactions to better experience the event.
Mindfulness through meditation is a tool that can promote acceptance and has played a central role in the new generation of therapies (Kabat-Zinn, 2001; Linehan, 1993). Mindfulness is an attempt to experience psychological phenomena unfettered by behavior or cognitions. It is the self-regulation of attention focused on the present moment with curiosity and openness (Bishop et al., 2007). Only very recently has research proposed or examined how therapies incorporating acceptance and mindfulness impact adherence. A book chapter promoted dialectical behavior therapy (a prominent intervention incorporating mindfulness) as a method for enhancing adherence to HIV regimens though no research was discussed therein (Singh & Ochitill, 2006). One study, an unpublished dissertation, examined the impact of an acceptance-based intervention on adherence to HIV medication (Moitra, 2009). Results of the randomized controlled trial did not show improvement in self-rated measures of adherence but did find enhanced immunity markers in the treatment, compared to control, group. Post hoc analyses showed baseline mindful acceptance predicted self-reported adherence.
Summary and Conclusions
The significant problem of treatment non-adherence was framed here as an important example of a broader collection of health-related decisions and behaviors. The rational patient was reviewed as one popular paradigm for understanding health decisions and behaviors including largely cognitive behavioral methods to impact the deliberative decision maker. These included shared decision making, motivational interviewing, and cognitive rehabilitation strategies that attempt to help the person with cognitive disability better grasp the decision process. We asserted, however, significant limitations to a deliberative perspective; the body of the paper described three. We also proposed ways in which these hypothetical explanations might be impacted to enhance satisfaction with health decisions and behaviors. (1) Instead of health decisions being made deliberatively through careful consideration of advantages and disadvantages, significant cognitive processes occur implicitly, potentially outside awareness. These might be simple associations between two constructs: taking medication is bad. Alternatively they may represent totally executive processes outside awareness, Dijksterhuis’ (2010) sleep on it effect. Theories and measurement models related to the Implicit Association Test and corresponding, practice-oriented clinical approaches have begun to describe and impact the implicit relationship between health decisions and satisfaction.
(2) The person is planful as well as influenced by the immediate exigencies, factors which often differ from the consideration of days earlier. Health behavior requires ongoing daily effort to realize a health plan. Research relying on experience sampling methods and ecological momentary interventions has helped to describe the immediacy of influences and ways to impact them. (3) A rational patient is not always a cool operator acting on decisions divorced from emotion. Instead research shows decision making often to be a hot process with emotions occurring both explicitly (as inhibitors of motivators for specific decisions) and implicitly (interoceptive and arousing processes that may orient the person to different decisions and behaviors). The evolution of clinical therapies to include mindfulness and acceptance may prove useful for improving satisfaction with health decisions.
Multi-Process Decision Making Model
Rational patient models are obviously limited such that health decision making needs a broader paradigm to fully understand choices people make about their psychiatric treatments. These need to carefully integrate supra-deliberative factors. Social psychologists in other venues have begun this by posing dual-process models that describe the interaction of explicit and implicit processes in everyday behavior (Chaiken & Trope, 1999). These models propose social decisions and behaviors as influenced by both deliberative processes and those that occur outside awareness. Subsequent research balances measures of implicit cognition with proxies of more rational processes. As these models matured, researchers identified factors that moderate the interaction of explicit and implicit cognitive processing. Fazio (1990; Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999; Olson & Fazio, 2008) outlined motivation and opportunity as determinants of implicit versus explicit effects on decision making. Given the effort that reflection requires in explicit processes, some motivation is required to be deliberative in behaviors, with the motivation to be accurate or the cost to judgmental mistakes prominent among these. When there are no perceived costs or benefits to a health behavior like taking medications, for example, there is no motivation to be deliberative or effortful in considerations. Fazio noted that cost to be deliberative is not sufficient for explicit processes. Time and resources to deliberate – opportunity -- must also be available. Situations requiring a rapid response fail to provide the time the person needs to carefully consider multiple contingencies. Hence, the person confronted by unexpected side effects of a medication moments before having to dash out of the apartment for work is more likely to rely on implicit processes.
Dual process models account for only one aspect of supra-deliberative factors so that a multi-process model (MPM) is actually needed to more completely explain health decisions. Additional processes as outlined here are immediate, instead of planful, and emotion-related instead of cold cognition. MPMs yield the exciting conundrum of theoretical paths that need to be tested to show how these various factors combine. Equally important are research programs that seek to test the relative and combined impact of strategies meant to target the various aspects of MPMs. Finally, comprehensive MPMs might lead to identifying whether attempts at fostering specific deliberative or supra-deliberative processes actually undermine health decisions. Does shared decision making, for example, interfere with the kinds of benefits that might occur from my automatic choices (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2010)?
MPMs do not limit progress to the processes identified herein. We expect to show additional theoretical constructs relevant to health decision making. For example, the rational patient model may suggest people acting alone in deciding about health options. Social psychology, however, has shown individual decisions only make sense in terms of the larger social network in which the person acts (Ajzen, 1988). Key to social network is typically family, close friends, and perhaps co-workers. Cultural variables are important moderators with, for example, collectivist cultures likely to influence the impact of social networks on individual health decisions differently than more individualist groups (Triandis & Gelfand, 2012). Additionally important to social network is the service provider as well as peers in the service system. The point here is that MPMs of health decision making provide a heuristic to go forward in more completely understanding how a person with psychiatric disability makes decisions about specific intervention options.
An additional goal of MPMs specific to the health decisions of people with psychiatric disabilities is ways in which these disabilities mediate the decision process. We alluded to the role cognitive disabilities have on decision making in some people with psychosis. Research needs to more fully examine how the supra-deliberative factors are impacted by these kinds of disabilities. For example, studies have recently examined the interaction of implicit processes and affective disturbances in people with disabilities in the schizophrenia spectrum (Cohen, Beck, Najolia, & Brown, 2011). Other areas of disability might also be important in advancing MPMs. Consider the impact of social networks of people with psychotic disorders on decision making. As a result, rehabilitation providers will be able to craft deliberative and supra-deliberative interventions that meet the specific challenges of those with serious mental illness.
Impact and Implications.
- Beyond the traditional model of shared decision making, the review outlines supra-deliberative processes that effect health actions: implicit cognitions, immediate influences, and emotional effects.
- This paper provides a summary of research and theory that represent supra-deliberative processes especially as related to psychiatric disabilities, and practices that might help people avail these processes when making health decisions. This calls for a multi-process model (MPM) of health decision making which is proposed here.
- These practices extend interventions beyond cognitive behavioral approaches to include strategies that impact implicit, immediate, and emotional processes.
Contributor Information
Patrick W. Corrigan, Illinois Institute of Technology
Nicolas Rüsch, Psychiatric University Hospital Zürich, Switzerland.
Dror Ben-Zeev, Dartmouth Medical School, Thresholds-Dartmouth Research Center.
Tamara Sher, Northwestern University.
References
- Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR. A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature. 2005;433:68–72. doi: 10.1038/nature03086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ajzen I. Attitudes, Personality, and Behavior. Milton-Keynes, England: Open University Pres; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Ajzen I, Fishbein M. Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Albarracin D, Johnson BT, Fishbein M, Muellerleile PA. Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:142–161. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.127.l.l42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armitage CJ, Conner M. Attitudinal ambivalence: A test of three key hypotheses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2000;26:1421–1432. doi: 10.1177/0146167200263009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baker B, Helmers K, O’Kelly B, Sakinofsky I, Abelsohn A, Tobe S. Marital cohesion and ambulatory blood pressure in early hypertension. American Journal of Cardiology. 1999;98:1069–1072. doi: 10.1016/S0895-7061(98)00184-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social learning theory. New York, NY: General Learning Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Bargh JA. The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In: Wyer RS, Srull TK, editors. Handbook of social cognition. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1994. pp. 1–40. [Google Scholar]
- Bauer MS. A review of quantitative studies of adherence to mental health clinical practice guidelines. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 2002;10(3):138–153. doi: 10.1080/10673220216217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275(5304):1293–1295. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck E, Cavelti M, Kvrgic S, Kleim B, Vauth R. Are we addressing the ‘right stuff’ to enhance adherence in schizophrenia? Understanding the role of insight and attitudes towards medication. Schizophrenia Research. 2011;132(1):42–49. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Zeev D, Young MA. Accuracy of hospitalized depressed patients’ and healthy controls’ retrospective symptom reports: An experience sampling study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2010;198(4):280–285. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181d6141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Zeev D, Young MA, Madsen JW. Retrospective recall of affect in clinically depressed individuals and controls. Cognition & Emotion. 2009;23(5):1021–1040. doi: 10.1080/02699930802607937. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Benazon NR, Foster MD, Coyne JC. Expressed emotion, adaptation, and patient survival among couples coping with chronic heart failure. Journal of Family Psychology. 2006;20(2):328–334. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, Devins G. Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice. 2004;11(3):230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blendy JA, Strasser A, Walters CL, Perkins KA, Patterson F, Berkowitz R, Lerman C. Reduced nicotine reward in obesity: Cross-comparison in human and mouse. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180:306–315. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2167-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broaddus M. Doctoral dissertation. 2009. Multidimensional implicit and explicit condom attitudes: Structure and implications for health messages. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI Num. AAT 3315812) [Google Scholar]
- Brownbridge G, Fielding D. Psychosocial adjustment and adherence to dialysis treatment regimes. Pediatric Nephrology. 1994;8:744–749. doi: 10.1007/BF00869109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caring J. Treating obesity: Interventions in an inner-city population. Psychiatric Services. 2008;59(10):1216–1217. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.59.10.1216-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celano MP, Linzer JF, Demi A, Bakeman R, Smith CO, Croft S, Kobrynski LJ. Treatment adherence among low-income, African American children with persistent asthma. Journal of Asthma. 2010;47(3):317–322. doi: 10.3109/02770900903580850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaiken S, Trope Y. Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Champion VL, Skinner CS. The Health Belief Model. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, editors. Health Behavior and Health Education. Theory, Research, and Practice. 4. San Francisco, CA: John Wiley and Sons, Inc; 2008. pp. 45–65. [Google Scholar]
- Cloutier J, Heatherton TF, Whalen PJ, Kelley WM. Are attractive people rewarding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:941–951. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen AS, Beck MR, Najolia GM, Brown LA. Affective disturbances in psychometrically defined schizotypy across direct, but not indirect assessment modes. Schizophrenia Research. 2011;128:136–142. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins FC, Green ED, Guttmacher AE, Guyer MS. A vision for the future of genomics research. Nature. 2003;422:835–847. doi: 10.1038/nature01626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Consedine NS, Magai C, Krivoshekova YS, Ryzewicz L, Neugut AI. Fear, anxiety, worry, and breast cancer screening behavior: A critical review. Cancer, Epidemiology, Biomarkers, and Prevention. 2004;13:501–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan PW. How stigma interferes with mental health care. American Psychologist. 2004;59:614–625. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan PW, Angell B, Davidson L, Marcus S, Salzer M, Kottsieper P, O’Connell M. From adherence to self-determination: Choosing to participate in self-evidence based practices. Psychiatric Services in press. [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan PW, Mueser KT, Bond GR, Drake RE, Solomon P, editors. The principles and practice of psychiatric rehabilitation: An empirical approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan PW, McCracken SG. Training teams to deliver better psychiatric rehabilitation programs. Psychiatric Services. 1999;50:43–46. doi: 10.1176/ps.50.1.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cota D, Tschöp MH, Horvath TL, Levine AS. Cannabinoids, opioids and eating behavior: The molecular face of hedonism? Brain Research Review. 2006;51(1):85–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Öhman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(2):189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R. Validity and reliability of the experience sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1987;175:526–536. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198709000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York, NY: Grosset/Putnam; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Dawkins L, Acaster S, Powell JH. The effects of smoking and abstinence on experience of happiness and sadness in response to positively valenced, negatively valenced and neutral film clips. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:425–431. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Depp C, Mausbach B, Granholm E, Cardenas V, Ben-Zeev D, Patterson T, Jeste D. Mobile interventions for severe mental illness. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2010;198:715–721. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181f49ea3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diiorio C, Mccarty F, Resnicow K, Holstad MM, Soet J, Yeager K, Lundberg B. Using motivational interviewing to promote adherence to antiretroviral medications: A randomized controlled study. Aids Care-Psychological and Socio-Medical Aspects of Aids/Hiv. 2008;20(3):273–283. doi: 10.1080/09540120701593489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dijksterhuis A, Aarts H. Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annual review of Psychology. 2010;61:467–490. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dijksterhuis A, Nordgren LF. A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2006;1:95–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00007.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiMatteo MR. Variations in patients’ adherence to medical recommendations: A quantitative review of 50 years of research. Medical Care. 2004;42(3):200–209. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000114908.90348.f9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiMatteo MR, Lepper HS, Croghan TW. Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment: A meta-analysis of the effects of anxiety and depression on patient adherence. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2000;160:2101–2107. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.14.2101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobkin PL, Liu A, Abrahamowicz M, Ionescu-Ittu R, Bernatsky S, Goldberger A, Baron M. Predictors of disability and pain six months after the end of treatment for fibromyalgia. Clinical Journal of Pain. 2010;26(1):23–29. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181b40ee6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drymalski WM, Campbell TC. A review of motivational interviewing to enhance adherence to antipsychotic medication in patients with schizophrenia: Evidence and recommendations. Journal of Mental Health. 2009;18(1):6–15. doi: 10.1080/09638230802052161. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dunton GF, Atienza AA, Castro CM, King AC. Using ecological momentary assessment to examine antecedents and correlates of physical activity bouts in adults age 50+ years: A pilot study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;38(3):249–255. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9141-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebner-Priemer UW, Eid M, Kleindienst N, Stabenow S, Trull TJ. Analytic strategies for understanding affective (in)stability and other dynamic processes in psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118(1):195–202. doi: 10.1037/a0014868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebner-Priemer UW, Kuo J, Welch S, Thielgen T, Witte S, Bohus M, Linehan MM. A valence-dependent group-specific recall bias of retrospective self-reports: A study of borderline personality disorder in everyday life. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2006;194:774–779. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000239900.46595.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards M, Davies M, Edwards A. What are the external influences on information exchange and shared decision-making in healthcare consultations: A meta-synthesis of the literature. Patient Education and Counseling. 2009;75(1):37–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein NB, Baucom DH. Enhanced cognitive-behavioral therapy for couples. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Fazio RH. Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 23. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1990. pp. 75–109. [Google Scholar]
- Fazio RH, Towles-Schwen T. The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes. In: Chaiken S, Trope Y, editors. Dual process theories in social psychology. New York, NY: Guilford; 1999. pp. 97–116. [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson ML, Katkin ES. Visceral perception, anhedonia, and emotion. Biological Psychology. 1996;42:131–145. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05151-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez-Duque D, Wifall T. Actor/observer asymmetry in risky decision making. Judgment and Decision Making. 2007;2(1):1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Freedman MJ, Lester KM, McNamara C, Milby JB, Schumacher JE. Cell phones for ecological momentary assessment with cocaine-addicted homeless patients in treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse and Treatment. 2006;30(2):105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman D, Hechter M. The contribution of Rational Choice Theory to macrosociological research. Sociological Theory. 1988;6:201–218. [Google Scholar]
- Friese M, Hofmann W, Wänke M. When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of implicit and explicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2008;47:397–419. doi: 10.1348/014466607X241540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fung V, Huang J, Brand R, Newhouse JP, Hsu J. Hypertension treatment in a Medicare population: Adherence and systolic blood pressure control. Clinical Therapy. 2007;29:972–984. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2007.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garbers S, Jessop DJ, Foti H, Uribelarrea M, Chiasson MA. Barriers to breast cancer screening for low-income Mexican and Dominican women in New York City. Journal of Urban Health. 2003;80(1):81–91. doi: 10.1093/jurban/jtg081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gattellari M, Butow PN, Tattersall MHN. Sharing decisions in cancer care. Social Science and Medicine. 2001;52(12):1865–1878. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00303-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gawronski B, Bodenhausen GV. Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilmer TP, Dolder CR, Lacro JP, Folsom DP, Lindamer L, Garcia P, Jeste DV. Adherence to treatment with antipsychotic medication and health care costs among medicaid beneficiaries with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161(4):692–699. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granholm E, Ben-Zeev D, Link PC, Bradshaw K, Holden J. Mobile assessment and treatment for schizophrenia (MATS): A pilot trial of an interactive text-messaging intervention for medication adherence, socialization, and auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Bulletin. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr155. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenfield S, Kaplan SH, Ware JE. Expanding patient involvement in care: Effects on patient outcomes. Annals of Internal Medicine. 1985;102(4):520–528. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-102-4-520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1464–1480. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwald AG, Nosek BA. Attitudinal dissociation: What does it mean? In: Petty RE, Fazio RH, Brinol P, editors. Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2009. pp. 65–82. [Google Scholar]
- Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann E, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;97(1):17–41. doi: 10.1037/a0015575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall EE, Ekkekakis P, Petruzzello SJ. Predicting affective responses to exercise using resting EEG frontal asymmetry: Does intensity matter? Biological Psychology. 2010;83(3):201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NL. Integrating the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination theory in health behaviour: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2009;14:275–302. doi: 10.1348/135910708X373959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison JA, Mullen PD, Green LW. A meta-analysis of studies of the Health Belief Model. Health Education Research. 1992;7:107–116. doi: 10.1093/her/7.1.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Barnes-Holmes D, Roche B, editors. Relational frame theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Strosahl K, Wilson KG. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Heron KE, Smyth JM. Ecological momentary interventions: Incorporating mobile technology into psychosocial and health behaviour treatments. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2009;15:1–39. doi: 10.1348/135910709X466063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Havermans RC, Wiers RW. Learning to dislike alcohol: Conditioning negative implicit attitudes toward alcohol and its effect on drinking behavior. Psychopharmacology. 2010;211:79–86. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1872-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Rothermund K, Wiers RW. Predicting alcohol use with a recoding-free variant of the Implicit Association Test. Addictive Behaviors. 2009;34:487–489. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Schoenmakers TM, Wiers RW. I didn’t feel like drinking but I don’t know why: The effects of evaluative conditioning on alcohol-related attitudes, craving and behavior. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:1161–1163. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hull CL. The conflicting psychologies of learning: A way out. Psychological Review. 1935;42:491–516. [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine. Methodological challenges in biomedical HIV prevention trials. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson NS, Christensen A, Prince SE, Cordova J, Eldridge K. Integrative behavioral couple therapy: An acceptance-based, promising new treatment for couple discord. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68(2):351–355. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.2.351. doi: 1100..11003377////00002222-006X.68.2.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jollant F, Lawrence NS, Olie E, O’Daly O, Malafosse A, Courtet P, Phillips ML. Decreased activation of lateral orbitofrontal cortex during risky choices under uncertainty is associated with disadvantageous decision-making and suicidal behavior. Neuroimage. 2010;51(3):1275–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones LE, Turvey C, Carney-Doebbeling C. Inadequate follow-up care for depression and its impact on antidepressant treatment duration among veterans with and without diabetes mellitus in the Veterans Health Administration. General Hospital Psychiatry. 2006;28:465–474. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2006.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joosten EA, DeFuentes-Merillas L, de Weert GH, Sensky T, van der Staak CP, de Jong CA. Systematic review of the effects of shared decision-making on patient satisfaction, treatment adherence and health status. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2008;77(4):219–26. doi: 10.1159/000126073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joosten EA, de Jong CA, de Weert-van Oene GH, Sensky T, van der Staak CP. Shared decision-making reduces drug use and psychiatric severity in substance-dependent patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2009;78(4):245–53. doi: 10.1159/000219524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness meditation for everyday life. London, UK: Piatkus; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D. Objective happiness. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. Well-being: Foundations of hedonic psychology. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation; 1999. pp. 3–25. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Fredrickson BL, Schrieber C, Redelmeier DA. When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychological Science. 1993;8:255–258. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00589.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kahler CW, Daughters SB, Leventhal AM, Gwaltney CJ, Palfai TP. Implicit associations between smoking and social consequences among smokers in cessation treatment. Behaviour Research & Therapy. 2007;45:2066–2077. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalsekar ID, Madhavan SS, Amonkar MM, Makela EH, Scott VG, Douglas SM, Elswick BL. Depression in patients with type 2 diabetes: Impact on adherence to oral hypoglycemic agents. Annals of Pharmacothetapy. 2006;40:605–611. doi: 10.1345/aph.1G606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kyrios M. Cognitive-behaviour therapy in medical illness. In: Simos G, editor. Cognitive-Behaviour Therapy: A guide for the practicing clinician. II. London, UK: Routledge; 2009. pp. 127–143. [Google Scholar]
- Lannin D, Matthews H, Mitchell J, Swanson M, Edwards M. Influence of socioeconomic and cultural factors on racial differences in late stage presentation of breast cancer. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1998;279:1801–1807. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.22.1801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin E, Levin A. Evaluation of spoken dialogue technology for real-time health data collection. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2006;8(4):e30. doi: 10.2196/jmir.8.4.e30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberman RP. Employment outcomes for SSA beneficiaries. Psychiatric Services. 2008;59(1):114–115. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.59.1.114-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindenberg S. Rational choice and sociological theory: New pressures on economics as a social science. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics. 1985;141:244–255. [Google Scholar]
- Linehan MM. Skills training manual for treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Liu LL, Park DC. Aging and medical adherence: The use of automatic processes to achieve effortful things. Psychological Aging. 2004;19:318–325. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludman E, Katon W, Bush T, Rutter C, Lin E, Simon G, Walker E. Behavioural factors associated with symptom outcomes in a primary care-based depression prevention intervention trial. Psychological Medicine: A Journal of Research in Psychiatry and the Allied Sciences. 2003;33(6):1061–1070. doi: 10.1017/S003329170300816X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magai C, Consedine NS, Neugut AI, Hershman DL. Common psychosocial factors underlying breast cancer screening and breast cancer treatment adherence: A conceptual review and synthesis. Journal of Women’s Health. 2007;16(1):11–23. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2006.0024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malm U, Ivarsson B, Allebeck P, Falloon IRH. Integrated care in schizophrenia: A 2-year randomized controlled study of two community-based treatment programs. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2003;107(6):415–423. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00085.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins RK, Mcneil DW. Review of motivational interviewing in promoting health behaviors. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29(4):283–293. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martire L, Schulz R, Helgeson V, Small B, Saghafi E. Review and meta-analysis of couple-oriented interventions for chronic illness. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2010;40(3):325–342. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9216-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCracken SG, Corrigan PW. Motivational interviewing for medication adherence in individuals with schizophrenia. In: Arkowitz H, Miller WR, editors. Motivational interviewing in the treatment of psychological problems. New York, NY: Guilford; 2008. pp. 249–276. [Google Scholar]
- Mela DJ. Novel food technologies: Enhancing appetite control in liquid meal replacements. Obesity. 2006;14:179–181. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Mitchenson L. Motivational interviewing in health care: Helping patients change behaviour. Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2009;14(1):55–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2008.00516_9.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moitra E. Doctoral dissertation. 2009. Acceptance-based intervention to promote HIV medication adherence. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI Num. AAT 3361936) [Google Scholar]
- Morgan K, Dixon S, Mathers N, Thompson J, Tomeny M. Psychological treatment for insomnia in the management of long-term hypnotic drug use: A pragmatic randomised controlled trial. British Journal of General Practice. 2003;53:923–928. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mount DL. Does cognition influence type 2 diabetes-related adherence? In: Shumaker SA, Ockene JK, Riekert KA, editors. The handbook of health behavior change. 3. New York, NY: Springer; 2009. pp. 693–707. [Google Scholar]
- Murray E, Davis H, Tai S, Coulter A, Gray A, Haines A. Randomised controlled trial of an interactive multimedia decision aid on benign prostatic hypertrophy in primary care. BMJ. 2001;323:493. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7311.493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naqvi NH, Bechara A. The hidden island of addiction: The insula. Trends in Neuroscience. 2009;32(1):56–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navarick DJ. Analysis of impulsive choice: Assessing effects of implicit instructions. The Psychological Record. 2004;54:505–522. [Google Scholar]
- Newby LK, LaPointe NM, Chen AY, Kramer JM, Hammill BG, DeLong ER, Califf RM. Long-term adherence to evidence-based secondary prevention therapies in coronary artery disease. Circulation. 2006;113(2):203–212. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.505636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicklas LB, Dunbar M, Wild M. Adherence to pharmacological treatment of non-malignant chronic pain: The role of illness perceptions and medication beliefs. Psychological Health. 2010;25(5):601–615. doi: 10.1080/08870440902783610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nock MK, Prinstein MJ, Sterba SK. Revealing the form and function of self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: A real-time ecological assessment study among adolescents and young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118:816–827. doi: 10.1037/a0016948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nosé M, Barbui C, Tansella M. How often do patients with psychosis fail to adhere to treatment programmes? A systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2003;33(7):1149–60. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703008328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson MA, Fazio RH. Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In: Petty RE, Fazio RH, Briñol P, editors. Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2009. pp. 19–64. [Google Scholar]
- Parsons JT, Golub SA, Rosof E, Holder C. Motivational interviewing and cognitive-behavioral therapy to improve HIV medication adherence among hazardous drinkers: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome. 2007;46(4):443–450. doi: 10.1097/qai.0b013e318158a461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Partridge AH, Wang PS, Winer EP, Avorn J. Nonadherence to adjuvant tamoxifen therapy in women with primary breast cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2003;21:602–606. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2003.07.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pescosolido B. Beyond rational choice: The social dynamics of how people seek help. American Journal of Sociology. 1992;97:1096–1138. doi: 10.1086/229863. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Transtheoretical therapy: Toward a more integrative model of change. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. 1982;19(3):276–288. [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, Di Clemente CC. Towards a comprehensive model of change. In: Miller WR, Heather N, editors. Treating addictive behaviours: Processes of change. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1986. pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
- Redelmeier DA, Kahneman D. Patient’s memories of painful medical treatments: Real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures. Pain. 1996;116:3–8. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(96)02994-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redelmeier DA, Katz J, Kahneman D. Memories of colonoscopy: A randomized trial. Pain. 2003;104:187–194. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00003-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid SC, Teesson M, Sannibale C, Matsuda M, Haber PS. The efficacy of compliance therapy in pharmacotherapy for alcohol dependence: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2005;66(6):833–41. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Remien RH, Stirratt MJ, Dolezal C, Dognin JS, Wagner GJ, Carballo-Dieguez A, Jung TM. Couple-focused support to improve HIV medication adherence: a randomized controlled trial. AIDS. 2005;19:807–81. doi: 10.1097/01.aids.0000168975.44219.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers RW. Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In: Cacioppo J, Petty R, editors. Social Psychophysiology. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1983. pp. 153–146. [Google Scholar]
- Rohrbaugh MJ, Shoham V, Coyne JC. Effect of marital quality on eight-year survival of patients with heart failure. American Journal of Cardiology. 2006;98:1069–1072. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2006.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenstock IM. Why people use health services. Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. 1966;44:94–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the Health Belief Model. Health Education Monographs. 1974;2:328–335. doi: 10.1177/109019817800600406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudnick A, Roe D, editors. Serious mental illness: person-centered approaches. Abingdon, UK: Radcliffe Publishing; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Rüsch N, Todd AR, Bodenhausen GV, Olschewski M, Corrigan PW. Automatically activated shame reactions and perceived legitimacy of discrimination: A longitudinal study among people with mental illness. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2010;41:60–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rüsch N, Todd AR, Bodenhausen GV, Weiden PJ, Corrigan PW. Implicit versus explicit attitudes toward psychiatric medication: Implications for insight and treatment adherence. Schizophrenia Research. 2009;112:119–122. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmaling KB, Blume AW, Afari N. A randomized controlled pilot study of motivational interviewing to change attitudes about adherence to medications for asthma. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings. 2001;8:167–172. doi: 10.1023/A:1011365519345. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Scott-Sheldon LA. Doctoral dissertation. 2006. Implicit-explicit ambivalent attitudes and health behaviors. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI Num. AAT 3221568) [Google Scholar]
- Shiffman S, Kirchner TR. Cigarette-by-cigarette satisfaction during ad libitum smoking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118(2):348–359. doi: 10.1037/a0015620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shumaker SA, Ockene JK, Riekert KA, editors. The handbook of health behavior change. 3. New York, NY: Springer; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Singh K, Ochitill H. Personality disorder and HIV infection. In: Fernandez F, Ruiz P, editors. Psychiatric Aspects of HIV/AIDS. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2006. pp. 101–110. [Google Scholar]
- Skinner BF. Science and human behavior. New York, NY: Macmillan; 1953. [Google Scholar]
- Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. The affect heuristic. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D, editors. Intuitive judgment: Heuristics and biases. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2002. pp. 397–420. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder DK, Castellani AM, Whisman MA. Current status and future directions in couple therapy. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:317–344. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KP, Christakis NA. Social networks and health. Annual Reviews of Sociology. 2008;34:405–429. [Google Scholar]
- Smith DC, Hall JA, Jang M, Arndt S. Therapist adherence to a Motivational-Interviewing intervention improves treatment entry for substance misusing adolescents with low problem perception. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2009;70:101–105. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sritharan R, Gawronski B. Changing implicit and explicit prejudice: Insights from the associative-propositional evaluation model. Social Psychology. 2010;41:113–123. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens A, Peschk I, Schwarz J. Implicit learning, executive function and hedonic activity in chronic polydrug abusers, currently abstinent polydrug abusers and controls. Addiction. 2007;102:937–946. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01823.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroebe W, Stroebe M. Social Psychology and Health. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Swendsen J, Ben-Zeev D, Granholm E. Real-time electronic ambulatory monitoring of substance use and symptom expression in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanis M. Health-related on-line forums: What’s the big attraction? Journal of Health Communication. 2008;13(7):698–714. doi: 10.1080/10810730802415316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor-Gooby P. Social divisions of trust: Scepticism and democracy in the GM nation? Journal of Risk Research. 2006;9(1):75–95. doi: 10.1080/13669870500288742. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Terra MB, Barros HMT, Stein AT, Figueira I, Athayde LD, Spanemberg L, da Silveira DX. Does co-occurring social phobia interfere with alcoholism treatment adherence and relapse? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2006;31:403–409. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2006.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thush C, Wiers RW. Explicit and implicit alcohol-related cognitions and the prediction of future drinking in adolescents. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:1367–1383. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thyrian JR, Freyer-Adam J, Hannöver W, Röske K, Mentzel F, Kufeld C, Hapke U. Adherence to the principles of motivational interviewing, clients’ characteristics and behaviour outcome in a smoking cessation and relapse prevention trial in women postpartum. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2297–2303. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Triandis HC, Gelfand MJ. In: Handbook of theories of social psychology. Val Lange PAM, Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET, editors. Vol. 2. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Ltd; 2012. pp. 498–520. [Google Scholar]
- Umeh K. Understanding adolescent health behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Mental health: A report of the Surgeon General—Executive summary. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- van Roosmalen MS, Stalmeier PFM, Verhoef LCG, Hoekstra-Weebers JEHM, Oosterwijk JC, Hoogerbrugge N, van Dall WAJ. Randomized trial of a decision aid and its timing for women being tested for a BRCA 1/2 mutation. British Journal of Cancer. 2004;90(2):333–342. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6601525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Servellen G, Chang B, Garcia L, Lombardi E. Individual and system level factors associated with treatment nonadherence in human immunodeficiency virus-infected men and women. AIDS Patient Care and STD’s. 2002;16(6):269–281. doi: 10.1089/10872910260066705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Velpry L. The patient’s view: Issues of theory and practice. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry. 2008;32(2):238–258. doi: 10.1007/s11013-008-9086-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waters AJ, Carter BL, Robinson JD, Wetter DW, Lam CY, Cinciripini PM. Implicit attitudes to smoking are associated with craving and dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;91:178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiens S, Mezzacappa ES, Katkin ES. Heartbeat detection and the experience of emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 2000;14:417–427. doi: 10.1080/026999300378905. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wiers RW, Rinck M, Kordts R, Houben K, Strack F. Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Addiction. 2010;105:279–287. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02775.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DM. Exercise, affect, and adherence: An integrated model and a case for self-paced exercise. Journal of Social and Experimental Psychology. 2008;30(5):471–496. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.5.471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams GC, Patrick H, Niemiec CP, Williams LK, Devine G, Lafata JE, Pladevall M. Reducing the health risks of diabetes: How self-determination theory may help improve medication adherence and quality of life. Diabetes Educator. 2009;35:484–492. doi: 10.1177/0145721709333856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willis J, Todorov A. First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science. 2006;17:592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills F. Beck’s cognitive therapy. Distinctive features. New York, NY: Routledge; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Winston JS, Strange BA, O’Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5:277–283. doi: 10.1038/nn816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. Adherence to long term therapies: Evidence for action. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Wykes T, Reeder C, Landau S, Everitt B, Knapp M, Patel A, Romeo R. Cognitive remediation therapy in schizophrenia: Randomized controlled trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;190:421–427. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.026575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]