Can Agitated Behavior of Nursing Home Residents with Dementia be Prevented With the Use of Standardized Stimuli? (original) (raw)

J Am Geriatr Soc. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 Aug 1.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC2955178

NIHMSID: NIHMS215230

Jiska Cohen-Mansfield, Ph.D.,1,2,3 Marcia S. Marx, Ph.D.,1 Maha Dakheel-Ali, M.D.,1 Natalie G. Regier, B.A.,1 Khin Thein, M.D.,1 and Laurence Freedman, PhD4

Jiska Cohen-Mansfield

1 Research Institute on Aging, Charles E. Smith Life Communities, Rockville, MD

2 George Washington University Medical Center, Washington, D.C

3 Tel-Aviv University’s Herczeg Institute on Aging, Tel Hashomer, Israel

Marcia S. Marx

1 Research Institute on Aging, Charles E. Smith Life Communities, Rockville, MD

Maha Dakheel-Ali

1 Research Institute on Aging, Charles E. Smith Life Communities, Rockville, MD

Natalie G. Regier

1 Research Institute on Aging, Charles E. Smith Life Communities, Rockville, MD

Khin Thein

1 Research Institute on Aging, Charles E. Smith Life Communities, Rockville, MD

Laurence Freedman

4 Gertner Institute, Institute for Epidemiology and Health Policy Research, Tel Hashomer, Israel

1 Research Institute on Aging, Charles E. Smith Life Communities, Rockville, MD

2 George Washington University Medical Center, Washington, D.C

3 Tel-Aviv University’s Herczeg Institute on Aging, Tel Hashomer, Israel

4 Gertner Institute, Institute for Epidemiology and Health Policy Research, Tel Hashomer, Israel

Corresponding author: Jiska Cohen-Mansfield, Ph.D., ABPP, Director, Research Institute on Aging, 6121 Montrose Road, Rockville, MD 20852, 301-770-8449, Fax: 301-770-8455, gro.emoh-werbeh@dleifsnam-nehoC

Abstract

Objectives

The objective of this paper was to assess the relative impact of different types of stimuli on agitated behaviors of nursing home residents with dementia.

Design Setting/Participants

Participants were 111 residents of 7 Maryland nursing homes with a diagnosis of dementia who exhibited agitation.

Intervention

Different types of stimuli (music, social stimuli, simulated social stimuli, and individualized stimuli based on the person’s self-identity) to prevent behavior problems.

Measurements

Agitation was directly observed and recorded via the Agitated Behaviors Mapping Instrument.

Results

All stimulus categories were associated with significantly less physical agitation than baseline observations, and all except for manipulative stimuli were associated with significantly less total agitation. Live social stimuli were associated with less agitation than music, self-identity, work, simulated social, and manipulative stimulus categories. Task and reading stimulus categories were each associated with significantly less agitation than work, simulated social, and manipulative stimulus categories. Music and self-identity stimuli were associated with less agitation than simulated social and manipulative stimuli.

Conclusion

Providing stimuli offers a proactive approach to preventing agitation in persons with dementia, with live social stimuli being most successful.

Keywords: dementia, agitation, prevention, stimuli, nursing home residents

INTRODUCTION

Distinct syndromes of agitation, also termed behavior problems, include aggressive behaviors, physical non-aggressive behaviors such as inappropriate manipulation of objects, and verbal agitation.1,2 When these types of behaviors are exhibited by people with dementia, caring for them is often overwhelming and cannot be managed in the community, and as a result, many are institutionalized. However, there is no guarantee that these behaviors will be managed any more effectively once agitated persons with dementia are institutionalized, as the focus in many nursing facilities is on completion of daily tasks rather than treatment of agitation. Clearly, there is a continuing need within the field of gerontology to identify ways to prevent and manage agitation in persons with dementia.

The pharmacological intervention approach posits that medication can be an effective treatment for agitation3, but the level of effectiveness is moderate at best,4,5 and the potential for side effects are substantial.69 In contrast, studies have shown that non-pharmacological interventions can be effective in decreasing agitation without the risk of the potential side-effects of medication, while simultaneously address the underlying unmet needs of the older person.10,11 As most nursing home residents spend much of their time unoccupied, a significant portion of their agitation is attributable to unmet needs related to boredom and confusion.12,13 Consequently, many nonpharmacological interventions offer a wide variety of ways to engage older persons with dementia and provide them with enjoyable ways to pass the time.

Music has shown positive effects in decreasing agitation.1416 These studies found that music successfully reduced aggressive and negative behaviors in various settings and during specific activities, such as bathing and mealtimes. Additionally, music therapy, which included singing, playing instruments, and dancing, was reported to result in a significant decrease in agitation.17,18

Several studies show animal-assisted therapy to be beneficial in decreasing agitation in people with dementia.1922 One study found a decrease in agitation when using simulated animal-assisted therapy, i.e., a stuffed or robotic animal23.

Social contact has been effective in decreasing agitation among people with dementia.24,25 When relatives or friends are not present, simulated interaction, which involves presenting either audiotapes or videotapes of family members to the nursing home resident, has been shown to decrease agitation.2629 Moreover, structured activities, which range from trivia games to puzzles, have also been shown to have a positive impact on lowering agitation.3033

Stimuli based on participants’ self-identity have also been found to lessen agitation. Cohen-Mansfield, Parpura-Gill, and Golander34 compared a control group to a treatment group, in which participants were engaged in activities corresponding to each one’s most salient self-identity. The treatment group showed a significant increase in interest, pleasure, and involvement in activities, fewer agitated behaviors during treatment, and increased orientation in the treatment period.

The purpose of the present study was to assess whether the systematic presentation of different types of stimuli could prevent agitation in a nursing home population. With the exception of live stimuli, all stimuli required minimal staff involvement. In general, psychosocial interventions have an effect on agitated behavior during the intervention, with little-to-no impact after its termination. Consequently, we limited this study to a systematic exploration of the effects of a variety of stimuli on behavior problems in the short-term. We compared the efficacy of different types of stimuli for preventing agitation, and based on the results of relevant existing literature, we hypothesized that: 1) all interventions would be preferable to no intervention and would offer a proactive approach to preventing agitation; 2) music, social stimuli, simulated social stimuli, and individualized stimuli based on the person’s self-identity would have a significant effect on the prevention of behavior problems; and 3) self-identity interventions would have the greatest effect in lowering agitation when compared to the other interventions, because each self-identity intervention is tailored to the distinct characteristics of the participant.

METHODS

Participants

Our original pool of participants was 193 residents with dementia from 7 Maryland nursing homes who had participated in a study in which they were presented with different types of stimuli35. For this paper, we selected only those who manifested at least minimal levels of agitated behavior (so that we could examine the effect of stimulus presentation on agitated behavior). The criterion for exhibiting minimal levels of agitation was quantified as an average of at least 0.5 behaviors per 3 minute observation (equivalent to a CMAI score of 42). This translates to a resident manifesting the verbally agitated behaviors of both negativism and strange noises at a rate of once or twice a day as well as manifesting cursing several times a week and hitting once or twice a week; or, a resident manifesting physically agitated behaviors of restlessness and pacing, both at a rate of several times a day, as well as manifesting the verbally agitated behavior of repeating sentences or questions at a rate of several times a week. We excluded 50 of the residents as they did not meet the criteria for agitated behavior, and excluded another 32 because they did not have data for at least one of the stimuli (typically because they refused the stimulus). Therefore, our final sample includes 111 study participants. A comparison of included and excluded participants is presented in Table 1. As can be seen, the included (agitated) participants differed significantly from the other two groups with respect to gender and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores36.

Table 1

Background characteristics of selected (agitated) participants, not selected (non-agitated) participants, and participants excluded due to missing data

Selected (agitated) n=111 Not selected (non-agitated) n=50 Missing data n=32
Age 85.4 ± 8.4 86.8 ± 6.0 88.5 ± 5.9 F(2,190)=2.322
Gender
Female 80.2% 66.0% 90.6% X2(2)=7.53*
Male 19.8% 34.0% 9.4%
White/Caucasian 78.4% 88.0% 81.3%
Ethnicity
African American 11.7% 6.0% 12.5% White verses other X2(2)=2.103
Asian/Pacific Islander 6.3% 4.0% 3.1%
Hispanic/Latino 1.8% 2.0% 3.1%
Native American 0.9% 0.0% 0.0%
Other 0.9% 0.0% 0.0%
Widowed 60.4% 71.4% 71.0%
Marital status
Married 20.7% 16.3% 22.6% Married verses other X2(2)=0.579
Divorced 11.7% 6.1% 0.0%
Never married 4.5% 6.1% 6.5%
Separated 2.7% 0.0% 0.0%
ADL performance 2.3 ± 1.0 2.4 ± 1.1 2.8 ± 1.0 F(2,189)=3.063*
MMSE score 5.1 ± 5.8 10.5 ± 6.3 9.0 ± 5.6 F(2,185)=15.869***

For the group of 111 participants, the average age was 85.4 years and 80.2% were female. The majority (78.4%) were Caucasian, and 60.4% of the participants were widowed. The average MMSE score was 5.

Assessments

Background assessment

Data pertaining to background variables were retrieved from the residents’ charts at the nursing homes by a trained research assistant, and included information about gender, age, marital status, medical information (medical conditions from which the resident suffers; a list of medications taken), and performance of activities of daily living (ADL; from the Minimum Data Set [MDS])37. ADL performance was assessed for 10 activities (bed mobility, transferring, locomotion on the unit, dressing, eating, toilet use, personal hygiene, bathing, bladder incontinence and bowel incontinence) utilizing a scale from 1 to 5 (with 5 representing maximum dependence), and a mean ADL score was calculated for each participant. All participants had a diagnosis of dementia. The MMSE was administered to each participant by a research assistant that was trained with regard to standardized administration and scoring procedures.

Self-identity

Whenever possible, we interviewed the resident and conducted a telephone interview with the closest relative of the participant, in which the Self-Identity Questionnaire (SIQ)38 was administered to determine what roles were important and enjoyable to the participant before the onset of dementia as well as what activities are currently enjoyed. This assessment examines four types of role-identity: professional, family-role, leisure activities, and personal attributes. Using the SIQ, we are able to prioritize role identities with respect to both the past and present (e.g., an identity that was very important in both the past and present would receive a higher priority than one that was important in only one timeframe). When there is a discrepancy among informants, we consider the participant’s level of cognitive functioning. If the resident is either unable to answer the questionnaire properly, or the responses are limited, the SIQ from the relative is given priority. We individualize interventions according to the data obtained through the SIQ, focusing on activities related to specific role identities.

Agitation

Direct observations were recorded via the Agitated Behaviors Mapping Instrument (ABMI)1, for which a trained research assistant recorded the frequency of occurrence of 14 items describing problem behaviors, characterized as physical agitation (e.g., pacing, repetitive behaviors) or verbal/vocal agitation (e.g., screaming, complaining, groaning, attention seeking). Inter-rater reliabilities regarding agitated behaviors for this instrument averaged 0.931.

Procedure

Informed consent was obtained for all study participants from their relatives or other responsible parties. Additional information on the informed consent process is available elsewhere39. Our main criterion for inclusion was a diagnosis of dementia. The criteria for exclusion were:

Once consent was obtained for eligible participants, background information was obtained from each participant’s chart in the nursing home. In addition, the MMSE was administered to each participant. Each participant was then presented with 25 different predetermined stimuli (Table 2) over a period of three weeks (approximately 4 stimuli per day). Stimuli were grouped into the following 8 categories: live social stimuli (included a real baby, a real dog, and one-on-one socializing with a research assistant), simulated social stimuli (included a life-like baby doll, a childish-looking doll, a plush animal, a robotic animal [approximately $78 from stores such as Toys R’Us], and a respite video40,41), manipulative stimuli (included a squeeze ball, a tetherball, an expanding sphere, an activity pillow, building blocks, a fabric book, a wallet for men/purse for women, and a puzzle), work related stimuli (included stamping envelopes, folding towels, and sorting envelopes), task related stimuli (included flower arrangement and coloring with markers), music stimulus (included only listening to music), reading stimulus (included only reading a large print magazine), and self-identity stimuli (included 2 individualized stimuli which were matched to each participant’s past identity with respect to occupation, hobbies, or interests). Self-identity stimuli therefore varied across participants, such that a book ledger could be given to a former accountant, while fabric samples could be presented to a former seamstress. With the exception of the self-identity stimuli, all stimuli were standardized across participants.

Table 2

Predetermined stimuli.

Stimulus category Stimuli used
Live social A real baby, a real dog, and one-on-one socializing
Task Flower arrangement and coloring with markers
Reading Reading a large print magazine
Self identity Individualized stimuli which were matched to each participant’s past identity with respect to occupation, hobbies, or interests
Music Listening to music
Work Stamping envelopes, folding towels, and sorting envelopes
Simulated social A life-like baby doll, a childish-looking doll, a plush animal, a robotic animal, a respite video
Manipulative A squeeze ball, a tetherball, an expanding sphere, an activity pillow, building blocks, a fabric book, a wallet for men/purse for women, and a puzzle
Baseline No stimulation provided\usual care

Baseline observations were performed daily for each study participant, prior to the initiation of the engagement trials, and consisted of a 3 minute observation in which the research assistant was not introduced to the study participant and no stimulus was presented. The timing of these observations was alternated, such that baseline would be conducted at the start of the morning session on Day 1 and then at the start of the afternoon session on Day 2, and so on. After baseline, a research assistant asked if the participant would like to engage in the activity (and, in the case of work related activities, if they would help the research assistant) and then left the room. We recorded whenever the participant refused the stimulus (through words or actions). A second research assistant, who remained unobtrusive, observed the participant and recorded agitation via the ABMI, entering the data directly onto a Palm Pilot Zire31™ using a specially designed program. Each trial lasted 3 minutes. Trials took place between 9:30 am–12:30 pm and between 2 pm–5:30 pm, as these are the times that residents are not usually occupied with care activities at the nursing home (e.g., meals in the dining room, bathing). Individual trials were separated by an intertrial interval of at least 5 minutes. The order of stimulus presentation was randomized for each participant.

Analytic approach

Because of the skewed distribution of the agitation scores, we used nonparametric statistics, and provide median levels and ranks (ranks are often used in nonparametric statistics and refer to ordering of items so that one is ranked higher, lower or equal to another item) for levels of agitation under different conditions. To illustrate levels of agitation in this sample, we divided the sample into quartiles (groups containing 25% of the total participants) based on agitation levels at baseline. By using quartiles, we were able to divide study participants into four defined levels based upon agitation values at baseline, and then describe the responses of each quartile to the stimuli or during baseline. For each quartile group we present median levels of total agitation during baseline and during presentation of each stimulus category. Given the skewed distribution of agitation scores, we used the nonparametric within-subject Friedman test to compare the effects of the different stimulus categories. Post hoc comparisons were conducted using the procedure described in Table 8 of Christensen, Ogden, Dunn, & Eggett42.

RESULTS

In Table 3, we present the median level of agitation at baseline (bottom row) for each group based on baseline quartiles as well as during the presentation of each stimulus category. As can be seen in the table, levels were very low for the first quartile group, and lower than baseline for 5 of the 8 stimulus categories. In contrast, for the other three quartile groups, agitation levels were higher, and all medians in the stimulus categories were lower than for baseline.

Table 3

Median levels for total agitation at baseline and during stimulus presentation for each stimulus group for four quartiles based on levels of agitation at baseline.

Baseline Group 1st Quartile 2nd Quartile 3rd Quartile 4th Quartile
Simulated Social 0.85 0.80 2.35 5.75
Manipulative 0.70 0.83 1.96 5.84
Music 0.50 1.00 0.00 6.00
Reading 0.00 0.00 0.75 5.50
Self-Identity 0.00 1.00 0.88 4.00
Work 0.67 0.67 2.25 4.92
Task 0.00 0.50 1.50 2.88
Live Social 0.00 0.00 0.00 4.00
Baseline 0.58 1.67 3.18 7.17

Friedman test analyses comparing levels of agitation for the 8 stimulus categories and baseline were statistically significant for total agitation (X2(8)= 76.8, p<0.001), verbal agitation (X2(8)= 75.5, p<0.001), and physical agitation (X2 (8)= 105.3, p<0.001; see Table 4). Post hoc analyses for total agitation revealed that all stimulus categories except for manipulative were associated with significantly less agitation than baseline observations. For physical agitation, the level of agitation was significantly lower for all stimulus categories in comparison to baseline. For verbal agitation, live social, task, reading, self identity, and music were associated with significantly less agitation than baseline. Additional examination of the post hoc analyses revealed a hierarchy among stimuli with regard to their effects on decreasing agitation. In the case of total agitation, live social, task, and reading stimuli did not significantly differ from each other, yet all 3 were significantly better (i.e., less agitation) than simulated social, manipulative stimuli and work. Music and self-identity stimuli had an intermediate position in the hierarchy as both were associated with significantly more agitation than live social stimuli (but not task or reading stimuli) yet significantly less agitation than manipulative stimuli, and self-identity stimuli were also associated with significantly less agitation than simulated social stimuli.

Table 4

Mean ranksa for the 8 stimulus categories and Baseline for total, verbal, and physical agitation and the resultant test statistics (n=111)

Mean Rank
Stimulus category Total agitation Physical agitation Verbal agitation
Live social 3.92** 4.12** 4.11**
Task 4.32** 4.07** 4.78*
Reading 4.43** 4.44** 4.31**
Self identity 4.66** 4.95** 4.69**
Music 4.79** 4.79** 4.35**
Work 5.26** 4.83** 5.49
Simulated social 5.51* 5.21** 5.83
Manipulative 5.72 5.70** 5.75
Baseline 6.40 6.88 5.69
Friedman Chi-Square X2(8)=76.8p<.001 X2(8)=105.3p<.001 X2(8)=75.5p<.001

DISCUSSION

The results of the present study demonstrate that exposure to any type of stimulus is preferable to current nursing home standards of care, confirming our first hypothesis. The second hypothesis was also supported in that music, live and simulated social stimuli, and individualized stimuli based on the person’s self-identity were all associated with decreased agitation. The third hypothesis, concerning the superiority of self-identity based stimulation over all other stimuli, was only partially supported. Only live stimulation was significantly superior to self-identity based stimulation, while self-identity stimulation was significantly superior to simulated social and to manipulative stimuli.

The results suggest that provision of stimuli is more effective for physical types of agitation than for verbal. This is congruent with prior findings that suggest that physical agitation is often the result of boredom43, a condition likely to be alleviated by stimuli, whereas verbal agitation has other etiologies (such as loneliness and pain), which require different approaches to prevention.

Live social stimuli were generally the most effective, but there was no clear “second-place” stimulus, as the categories of task, reading, music, and self-identity are all comparable. Work and simulated social stimuli seemed to be somewhat less effective, and the category associated with the least decrease in agitation relative to baseline was manipulative stimuli.

Self-identity based stimuli were significantly superior to some of the other stimulus categories (e.g., manipulative, simulated social), but were not found to be significantly different than several other categories of stimuli. They were significantly inferior only to live social stimuli, which are known to be extremely potent. Self-identity stimuli are therefore considered to have been relatively effective for the current sample. As for manipulative interventions, while these were indeed the least effective, these were nonetheless significantly superior to the no stimulus condition for physical agitation, most likely addressing a need for physical stimulation.

While the results showed a clear superiority of provision of stimuli over baseline of regular care, the hierarchy among the stimulus categories was much less clear, for which there are several possible explanations. First, despite meeting the criteria for manifestation of agitation in order to be included in this analysis, most participants were not agitated most of the time. Second, each category of stimuli includes several specific stimuli which are conceptually similar, but may or may not have the same impact on agitation. Third, the stimuli may have a distinct impact on different individuals based on their prior habits or current abilities. For example, the work-related stimulus of folding towels may be meaningful for a former homemaker, while the work-related stimulus of sorting envelopes would not have the same effect. Therefore, interventions truly do need to be individualized to a greater extent than just matching persons to categories that seem appropriate. Individualizing interventions according to cognitive functioning can play a role in the utility of stimuli, as our prior research found that participants with comparatively higher levels of cognitive functioning were more likely to spend time engaged in work activities, whereas those with low levels of cognitive functioning were more engaged by simulated social stimuli, which do not require active responses44. Functional and sensory limitations should also be taken into account when tailoring interventions to an individual.

The present study has several limitations. The sample of participants was chosen based on the criteria of being a nursing home resident and having a diagnosis of dementia, and was not based on clinical problems. Therefore, despite selection based on minimal levels of agitation, participants were not highly agitated most of the time and were generally less agitated than in studies where persons were specifically selected for agitation45. We believe that these relatively low levels of agitation presented a floor effect and may have obscured and minimized the impact of the stimuli, thereby preventing us from presenting meaningful differences among individual stimuli. The relationship between agitation and dementia is complex, and nursing home residents with dementia manifest agitation in unique and varying degrees. The broad spectrum of agitation severity was not fully captured in this sample, and future research is needed to replicate the study in a sample that is more highly agitated. Another limitation is that the observation of agitation was brief, as only the immediate reaction (within the first 3 minutes) was recorded. Although agitation duration varies for each individual and within different contexts, persons with dementia frequently experience lengthy periods of agitation and the three minute observation cutoff may not allow for a comprehensive picture of individual agitation. Future research needs to ascertain whether populations with more extreme levels of agitation have similar responses to stimuli. Furthermore, when participants refused stimuli we did not record their level of agitation. Future researchers should examine agitation over longer periods and explore the relationship between refusals and the degree of agitation.

This study adds to the understanding of the ability of various stimuli to mollify and prevent agitated behaviors in persons with dementia. It is important to explore the effectiveness of these and other stimuli as non-pharmacological interventions before turning to medication, given the potentially harmful side-effects.4,6,8,10,11,46 The findings have implications to practice in that they indicate that even the simple provision of stimuli may be useful in preventing agitation. Nursing home units with persons with dementia need to have stimuli appropriate for the residents available and need to train staff members in how to provide them as one of the practices for preventing agitation among residents with dementia. It is anticipated that further exploration of a range of stimuli and tailoring them to abilities and preferences will further enhance the preventive effect of such stimuli.

Acknowledgments

We thank the nursing home residents, their relatives, and the staff members and administration of the nursing homes for all of their help, without which this study would not have been possible.

This study was supported by National Institutes of Health grant AG R01 AG021497.

Sponsor’s Role: None.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest: The editor in chief has reviewed the conflict of interest checklist provided by the authors and has determined that the authors have no financial or any other kind of personal conflicts with this paper.

Author Contributions:

  1. JC-M: Conceptualized and designed study, analyzed and interpreted data, assisted with writing and editing the manuscript.

  2. MSM: Conceptualized and designed study, analyzed and interpreted data, assisted with writing and editing the manuscript.

  3. MD: Assisted with acquisition of subjects, collected data, analyzed and interpreted data.

  4. NGR: Assisted with acquisition of subjects, collected data, assisted with writing and editing the manuscript.

  5. KT: Assisted with acquisition of subjects, collected data, assisted with data analysis.

  6. LSF: Assisted with analyzing and interpreting data and writing and editing the manuscript.

References

1. Cohen-Mansfield J, Werner P, Marx MS. An observational study of agitation in agitated nursing home residents. Int Psychogeriatr. 1989;1:153–165. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Rabinowitz J, Davidson M, De Deyn PP, et al. Factor analysis of the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory in three large samples of nursing home patients with dementia and behavioral disturbance. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13:991–998. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

4. Ballard C, Lana MM, Theodoulou M, et al. A randomized, blinded, placebo-controlled trial in dementia patients continuing or stopping neuroleptics (The DART-AD Trial) PLOS Medicine. 2008;5 Article e76. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. Rosenheck RA, Leslie DL, Sindelar JL, et al. Cost-benefit analysis of second-generation antipsychotics and placebo in a randomized trial of the treatment of psychosis and aggression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1259–1268. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Citrome L. Atypical antipsychotics, elderly patients, and mortality risk. South Med J. 2007;100:964–965. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Gill SS, Bronskill SE, Normand SL, et al. Antipsychotic drug use and mortality in older adults with dementia. Ann Int Med. 2007;146:775–786. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Jeste DV, Blazer D, Casey D, et al. ANCP white paper: Update on use of antipsychotic drugs in elderly persons with dementia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:957–970. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Madhusoodanaan S, Shah P, Brenner R, et al. Pharmacological treatment of the psychosis of Alzheimer’s disease: What is the best approach? CNS Drugs. 2007;21:101–115. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Cohen-Mansfield J. Nonpharmacological interventions for inappropriate behaviors in dementia: A review, summary, and critique. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;9:361–381. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Cohen-Mansfield J, Mintzer J. Time for change: The role of nonpharmacological interventions in treating behavior problems in nursing home residents with dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2005;19:37–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Burgio LD, Scilley K, Hardin JM, et al. Studying disruptive vocalization and contextual factors in the nursing home using computer-assisted real-time observation. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1994;49:P230–P239. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

13. Ice GH. Daily life in a nursing home: Has it changed in 25 years? J Aging Stud. 2002;16:345–359. [Google Scholar]

14. Gerdner LA. Effects of individualized versus classical ‘relaxation’ music on the frequency of agitation in elderly persons with Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders. Int Psychogeriatr. 2000;12:49–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Hicks-Moore SL. Relaxing music at mealtime in nursing homes: Effects of agitated patients with dementia. J Gerontol Nurs. 2005;31:26–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Sherratt K, Thornton A, Hatton C. Emotional and behavioural responses to music in people with dementia: An observational study. Aging Ment Health. 2004;8:233–241. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Brotons M, Pickett-Cooper P. The effects of music therapy intervention on agitation behaviors of Alzheimer’s disease patients. J Music Ther. 1996;33:2–18. [Google Scholar]

18. Svansdottir HB, Snaedal J. Music therapy in moderate and severe dementia of Alzheimer’s type: a case-control study. Int Psychogeriatr. 2006;18:613–621. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Churchill M, Safaoui J, McCabe BW, et al. Using a therapy dog to alleviate the agitation and desocialization of people with Alzheimer’s Disease. J Psychosoc Nurs. 1999;37:16–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Fritz CL, Farver TB, Kass PH, et al. Association with companion animals and the expression of noncognitive symptoms in Alzheimer’s patient. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1995;183:459–463. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. Kanamori M, Suzuki M, Yamamoto K, et al. A day care program and evaluation of animal-assisted therapy (AAT) for the elderly with senile dementia. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2001;16:234–239. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Zisselman MH, Rovner BW, Shmuely Y, et al. Pet therapy intervention with geriatric psychiatry patients. Am J Occup Ther. 1996;50:47–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Libin A, Cohen-Mansfield J. Therapeutic robocat for nursing home residents with dementia: A comparative study. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2004;19:111–116. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Cohen-Mansfield J, Werner P. Management of verbally disruptive behaviors in nursing home residents. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1997;52A:M369–M377. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Runci S, Doyle C, Redman J. An empirical test of language-relevant interventions for dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 1999;11:301–311. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Woods P, Ashley J. Simulated presence therapy: Using selected memories to manage problem behaviors in Alzheimer’s disease. Geriatr Nurs. 1995;16:9–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Camberg L, Woods P, Ooi WL, et al. Evaluation of simulated presence: A personalized approach to enhance well-being in persons with Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47:446–452. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Werner P, Cohen-Mansfield J, Fischer J, et al. Characterization and effectiveness of family-generated videotape for the management of VDB. J Appl Gerontol. 2000;19:42–57. [Google Scholar]

29. Garland K, Beer E, Eppingstall B, et al. A comparison of two treatments of agitated behavior in nursing home residents with dementia: simulated presence and preferred music. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15:514–521. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Groene RW. Effectiveness of music therapy 1:1 intervention with individuals having senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. J Music Ther. 1993;30:138–157. [Google Scholar]

31. Aronstein A, Olsen R, Shulman E. The nursing assistants use of recreational interventions for behavioral management of residents with Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 1996;11:26–31. [Google Scholar]

32. Sival RC, Vingerhoets RW, Haffmans PM, et al. Effect of a program of diverse activities on disturbed behavior in three severely demented patients. Int Psychogeriatr. 1997;9:423–430. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

33. Kolanowski AM, Litaker M, Buettner L. Efficacy of theory-based activities for behavioral symptoms of dementia. Nurs Res. 2005;54:219–228. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Cohen-Mansfield J, Golander H, Arnheim G. Self-identity in older persons suffering from dementia: Preliminary results. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51:381–394. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

35. Cohen-Mansfield J, Dakheel-Ali M, Marx MS. Engagement in persons with dementia: The concept and its measurement. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;17:299–307. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. Folstein M, Folstein S, McHugh P. Mini-mental state: A practical method of grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

37. Morris J, Hawes C, Murphy K, et al. MDS Resident Assessment. Natick, MA: Eliot Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]

38. Cohen-Mansfield J, Parpura-Gill A, Golander H. Utilization of self-identity roles for designing interventions for persons with dementia. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2006;61:202–212. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

39. Cohen-Mansfield J, Kerin P, Pawlson LG, et al. Informed consent for research in the nursing home: Processes and results. Gerontologist. 1988;28:355–359. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Hall L, Hare J. Video respite™ for cognitively impaired persons in nursing homes. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 1997;12:117–121. [Google Scholar]

41. Lund DA, Hill RD, Caserta MS, et al. Video respite(™): An innovative resource for family, professional caregivers, and persons with dementia. Gerontologist. 1995;35:683–687. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Christensen ZT, Ogden LV, Dunn ML, et al. Multiple comparison procedures for analysis of ranked data. J Food Sci. 2006;71:132–143. [Google Scholar]

43. Cohen-Mansfield J. The language of behavior. In: Downs M, Bowers B, editors. Excellence in Dementia Care. England: Open University Press; 2008. pp. 187–211. [Google Scholar]

44. Cohen-Mansfield J, Marx MS, Dakheel-Ali M, et al. Can persons with dementia be engaged with stimuli? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18:351–362. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

45. Cohen-Mansfield J, Libin A, Marx MS. Non-pharmacological treatment of agitation: A controlled trial of systematic individualized intervention. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62:908–916. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

46. Douglas IJ, Smeeth L. Exposure to antipsychotics and risk of stroke: Self controlled case series study. Br Med J. 2008;337:a1227. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]