negativity bias - The Skeptic's Dictionary (original) (raw)
From Abracadabra to Zombies | View All
The evil that men do lives after them; The good is oft interred with their bones. --Marc Antony, Julius Caesar by Shakespeare (act 3, scene ii)
I hate losing more than I love winning. --Billy Beane
Good evolutionary reasons exist for negativity bias given that negative events can be much more costly in fitness terms than positive events are beneficial; to state the obvious, infection, injury, and death curtail reproductive opportunities. --Hibbing et al.
__________
Brief contact with a cockroach will usually render a delicious meal inedible. The inverse phenomenon—rendering a pile of cockroaches on a platter edible by contact with one’s favorite food—is unheard of." So begins a classic paper by Paul Rozin and Edward B. Royzman: "Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion" (2001).
You might think you're weird when a thousand good things happen but you focus on the one bad thing. You're not. That's the way our brains are hardwired. We're designed by nature to pay more attention and react more quickly and more strongly to negative than to positive news. One salient misdeed by a person will often outweigh years of good works. Years of building up a positive image can be destroyed in an instant by a single misstep. This tendency to give more weight to the negative is called negativity bias and is defined as "the propensity to attend to, learn from, and use negative information far more than positive information." Our brain evolved to react more quickly to fear than to hope, to respond to a threat more quickly and more intensely than to an opportunity for pleasure. And this trait has carried over into modern times in ways that are not always beneficial.
Daniel Kahneman writes:
The brains of humans and other animals contain a mechanism that is designed to give priority to bad news. By shaving a few hundredths of a second from the time needed to detect a predator, this circuit improves the animal’s odds of living long enough to reproduce. (Thinking, Fast and Slow, p. 301.)
However, negativity bias makes us vulnerable to manipulation by those who would play on our fears. For example, National Security Advisor Condoleezza Rice may not have had any evidence that Saddam Hussein had weapons of mass destruction, but she could put the fear of god into many people just by warning us that the "smoking gun of evidence for WMDs in Iraq could come in the form of a mushroom cloud."
Loss aversion is another way that negativity bias manifests itself. Paul Rozin writes in "Bad is Stronger Than Good":
Bad emotions, bad parents, and bad feedback have more impact than good ones, and bad information is processed more thoroughly than good. The self is more motivated to avoid bad self-definitions than to pursue good ones. Bad impressions and bad stereotypes are quicker to form and more resistant to disconfirmation than good ones. (Quoted in Daniel Kahneman's Thinking, Fast and Slow, p. 302.)
Potential losses affect us more deeply than potential gains. This can lead to some irrational behavior, as is evidenced by those many times that we pass up an opportunity to benefit either financially or psychologically because we are afraid to take the risk of a loss. Long term investors who put large chunks of their portfolio in bonds are a prime example. Over the past 80 years, stocks have provided a 6.5% return (adjusted for inflation), while bonds have returned 0.5% (Lehrer, Jonah. How We Decide, p. 77). Bonds are considered a safer investment because there is a greater chance of losing money in stocks. For many people, the chance of losing money has a larger effect on their decision making than the chance of gaining money by investing in what may be riskier in the short run but more profitable in the long run. Most people, if offered a chance to win 150orlose150 or lose 150orlose100 on a coin toss won't take the deal. The potential loss, though less than the potential gain by a substantial amount, isn't worth the risk.
Loss aversion may explain why Pascal's wager seems reasonable to many people. People don't want to take the risk of losing eternal life by not believing in the god of Abraham. The safe bet is to believe. The 17th century mathematician argued that it would be wise to believe in the god of Abraham because you risk eternal life by not believing and if this god doesn't exist, you lose nothing in comparison to eternal life. If eternal life with this god is not attractive to you, then the potential loss by not believing isn't likely to affect you much. Also, if you have no fear of Hell (i.e., eternal suffering of some sort) because you consider its actual existence to be near zero in probability, then it is unlikely that loss aversion will drive you to believe in this god, even though your wager is just your life, i.e., you must act as if this god exists. On the other hand, the general principle behind the wager seems sensible: only a fool wouldn't wager next to nothing when the prize, if you win, is of infinite value. You might not bet 100forachancetowin100 for a chance to win 100forachancetowin150 on a coin toss, but you would be a fool not to bet 1onachancetowin,say,1 on a chance to win, say, 1onachancetowin,say,1,000,000 on a coin toss.
One effect of negativity bias is that we are likely to give more credence and more weight to negative claims about positions or candidates that we oppose than we are to positive claims about them. We are likely to not be very critical in our examination of such negative claims, certainly not as critical as when negative claims are made against views we cherish.
Another effect of negativity bias is that we are likely to be afraid of things disproportionately to the evidence, e.g., most people who are afraid of flying in airplanes have little fear of driving in an automobile even though their chance of being killed in an automobile crash is much higher than their chance of being killed in an airplane crash.
Negativity bias manifests itself in various ways involving contagion. For example, a person of the Brahmin (priestly) caste can be sullied by contact with a member of the Shudra (servant) class, but Shudras are not purified or elevated in status by contact with the Brahmins. Rozin and Royzman write:
The contamination often occurs by eating food prepared by a lower caste. On the other hand, when people of lower castes consume foods prepared by higher castes, there is no corresponding elevation in their status. Stevenson summarized this feature of the caste system with the phrase “pollution always overcomes purity” ("Status evaluation in the Hindu caste system." Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 84, p. 50).
On the other hand, an argument has been made for a positivity bias in The Pollyanna Principle: Selectivity in Language, Memory, and Thought by Margaret Matlin and David Stang (1978). Matlin and Stang claimed that their research showed that people are more likely to expose themselves to positive stimuli than they are to avoid negative stimuli and that they encounter more positive stimuli than negative stimuli,* which seems intuitively what you'd expect from pleasure-seeking animals.
If you are familiar with the Forer effect, you know that people tend to agree with positive statements made about themselves (whether they're true or not) and these kinds of statements are more likely to be accepted than negative statements about themselves. Studies on self-deception consistently find most people overestimate their possession of positive traits. So, when it comes to evaluating oneself, the negativity bias seems to be overpowered by the positivity bias.
further reading
Hansen, Rick, "Confronting the Negativity Bias," Huff Post.
new Hibbing, John R. et al. "Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology." BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2014) 37, 297–350.
Mooney, Chris. "Scientists Are Beginning to Figure Out Why Conservatives Are…Conservative."Mother Jones. July 15, 2014.[/new]
Vaish, Amrisha, Tobias Grossmann and Amanda Woodward, "Not All Emotions Are Created Equal: The Negativity Bias in Social–Emotional Development," Psychological Bulletin, 2008, Vol. 134, No. 3, 383–403.
Last updated 20-Jul-2014