Dan Villarreal | University of Canterbury/Te Whare Wānanga o Waitaha (original) (raw)
Talks by Dan Villarreal
New Zealand Linguistic Society 2019, 2019
New Zealand Linguistic Society, 2018
Southland English has historically been New Zealand’s only (partially) rhotic variety. There has ... more Southland English has historically been New Zealand’s only (partially) rhotic variety. There has only been one large-scale study of Southland (r), which suggested a resurgence of rhoticity following NURSE among young women (Bartlett 2002). We build on this work, using modern statistical methods to better understand the linguistic and social conditioning of change in Southland (r).
We analysed over 20,000 tokens of non-prevocalic Southland (r), coded as present/absent. 20% of tokens were hand-coded; the rest were automatically coded via a random-forest classifier trained on the hand-coded tokens to predict (r) presence/absence based on 180 acoustic measures (this auto-coder achieved over 80% accuracy on the hand-coded training set). Data were modelled via logistic mixed-effects regression, with a three-way generation distinction (birth years 1900–30, 1931–55, 1956–80).
As expected, this analysis reveals a significant effect for vowel, with greater rhoticity for NURSE than other vowels, and a significant effect for generation, which indicates a change in apparent time. The statistical modelling allows us to see further fine-grained phonological and grammatical conditioning of the change as it progressed throughout the speech community. For instance, we find that NURSE before non-sibilant fricatives lags in rhoticity among the oldest speakers but catches up to other NURSE environments among middle and young speakers. We also find that the increase of rhoticity in NURSE appeared first in content words, then spread to function words. We discuss the full trajectory of change for Southland (r) and highlight some implications for theories of phonological change more generally.
Experimental studies of priming have shown that we have a strong tendency to repeat linguistic ma... more Experimental studies of priming have shown that we have a strong tendency to repeat linguistic material that we have recently produced, seen or heard (Bock 1986; Bock & Branigan et al 2000). A growing body of work from corpus linguistics and sociolinguistics suggests that this also happens in more natural types of speech (Gries 2005; Szmrecsanyi 2006; Poplack 1980; Travis 2007). The idea that speakers use clusters of similar variants in natural running speech has been reported before in sociolinguistics but the suggestion has been that repetition is under the control of the speaker as they engage in the construction and maintenance of sociolinguistic style (Eckert and Rickford 2001). This paper is part of a recent attempt to open up an intriguing possibility: that previous work invoking style as an explanation could perhaps be equally well, if not better, explained by models of priming (Tamminga (2016), Clark (2014, 2018).
This paper examines evidence for within-speaker repetition effects by exploring data from a corpus of NZ English monologues. We present data from 4 phonological variables: word medial intervocalic /t/, KIT, DRESS and STRUT. By fitting mixed effect regression models incorporating both the realisation of the preceding variable, and a range of other linguistic and social predictors, we show two main findings that are of interest to sociolinguistics:
• Speakers show a repetition effect reminiscent of priming for those variables undergoing change (i.e. medial /t/, KIT and DRESS). This has methodological implications: it could mean that a proportion of the variation that has previously been attributed to sociolinguistic factors could potentially be attributed to more mechanistic repetition effects.
• In the case of medial /t/, priming interacts with the speaker’s gender. No psycholinguistic work predicts different priming behaviour for males and females and most sociolinguistic work assumes that men and women from the same speech community will share the same ‘internal’ constraints on variation.
We therefore argue that this work benefits sociolinguistics by suggesting that phonological variation is responsive not only to style but also to psycholinguistic factors like priming.
Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) have been found throughout the American... more Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) have been found throughout the American West (Fridland et al. 2016). One CVS feature, TRAP backing, is associated with California and Californian values (Villarreal 2016). It remains to be seen what regional identities or values TRAP backing indexes in communities undergoing the CVS outside of California. This study investigates the local construction of meaning for a supra-local sound change by examining how listeners in the Midwestern state of Kansas, which is undergoing front lax vowel retraction, perceive TRAP backing.
Fifty-one university students participated in a matched-guise task featuring stimuli read by young Kansan speakers. In each trial, listeners rated speakers on 14 Likert scales. Four critical stimuli belonged to one of two matched guises, which were acoustically manipulated such that only TRAP F2 differed between matched guises. Conservative guises contained fronted TRAP and shifted guises contained backed TRAP.
A principal components analysis of ratings revealed three principal components: PC1, measuring “general prestige,” PC2, measuring “Kansan-ness,” and PC3, measuring “innovativeness.” PC1 and PC3 significantly correlated with guise (p < .05), with shifted guises rated higher on “general prestige” and “innovativeness.” Conversely, PC2 significantly correlated with listeners’ regional identifications (p < .001), as stimuli rated high for “Kansan-ness” were most likely to be identified as being from Kansas and least likely to be identified as being from New York or California.
These results suggest that instead of associating TRAP backing with local identity, as in California, this sound change appears to index prestige and youth in Kansas, perhaps motivating the spread of this sound change in the region. These results provide clues to the rapid spread of the CVS, highlighting the local construction of meaning for a supra-local sound change.
This matched-guise experiment assessed whether sociolinguistic judgments are subject to increment... more This matched-guise experiment assessed whether sociolinguistic judgments are subject to incrementality, with judgments increasing in magnitude as variable stimuli demonstrate more extreme differences in some critical dimension. In particular, this task tested whether it is possible to trigger judgments of Barack Obama as “more” or “less” black (e.g., Alim & Smitherman 2011) by manipulating his intonational contours.
Stimuli were derived from twenty declarative Intonational Phrases with H* and/or L+H* pitch accents (following ToBI conventions for Mainstream American English of Beckman and Ayers 1997). Ten phrases featured L+H* pitch accents—which occur more frequency in African American Language (AAL) (Holliday 2016)—and ten featured H* pitch accents. Four stimuli apiece were created from these twenty phrases by using Praat to make each pitch accent more extreme by semitone-based F0 steps, yielding a four-way Manipulation Step factor. Seventy-nine American English listeners rated these stimuli in randomized order by responding to the question “How black does Obama sound here?”.
A mixed-effects regression model of ratings assessed the interaction of Pitch Accent type, Manipulation Step, and Boundary Tone, with Phrase nested within Pitch Accent. No main effect for Pitch Accent type emerged, indicating no rating differences for L+H* and H* phrases overall (p=.27), despite the higher likelihood of occurrence of L+H* in AAL. However, the interaction of Pitch Accent and Manipulation Step was significant, with more extreme L+H* phrases rated as more black (ps<.05); no such effect was observed among manipulation steps for H*-containing phrases. No listener demographic factors (e.g., listener race, gender, political affiliation) significantly affected blackness ratings.
These results indicate that listeners’ sociolinguistic perceptions are sensitive to the magnitude of the input, a finding that indicates promising directions for research in language attitudes and sociolinguistic cognition.
Suprasegmental features, especially intonation and voice quality, may be of special interest to s... more Suprasegmental features, especially intonation and voice quality, may be of special interest to sociolinguists as well as the public, due to their high level of salience for listeners (Purnell et al. 1999, Baugh 2015, Thomas 2015, Holliday 2016). Nevertheless, suprasegmentals have received little in the way of perceptual study. Building on the findings of Alim and Smitherman (2011), who assert that Barack Obama often employs a unique intonational style that appeals to both white and black audiences, this matched-guise experiment tested whether it is possible to trigger judgments of Obama as “more” or “less” black by manipulating aspects of his intonation. Moreover, this experiment assessed whether sociolinguistic judgments are subject to incrementality, with judgments increasing in magnitude as the variable stimuli demonstrate more extreme differences in F0.
This study builds on as well as intonational methodologies by testing listener judgments of manipulations of Obama’s utterances in one interview. The utterances were declarative Intonational Phrases with H* and/or L+H* pitch accents (following ToBI conventions for Mainstream American English of Beckman and Ayers 1997). Ten phrases featured L+H* pitch accents—which occur more frequency in AAL (McLarty 2011, Thomas 2015, Holliday 2016)— and ten featured H* pitch accents. Four critical stimuli apiece were created from these twenty phrases by using Praat to make each pitch accent more extreme by F0 steps (i.e., H* accents were raised at maximum F0 by one-semitone steps, and L+H* accents were raised at maximum F0 by one-semitone steps and lowered at minimum F0 by half-semitone steps), leading to a fourway Manipulation Step factor. Seventy-nine American English listeners heard these 80 critical stimuli, with 40 fillers, in randomized order in an online experiment. For each trial, listeners responded to the question “How black does Obama sound here?” using a 100-point slider bar.
A mixed-effects regression model of normalized ratings assessed the interaction of Pitch Accent type, Manipulation Step, and Boundary Tone, with Phrase nested within Pitch Accent. No main effect for Pitch Accent type was found, indicating that, overall, L+H* phrases are not rated more or less black than H* phrases (p=.27), despite the higher likelihood of occurrence of L+H* in AAL. However, the model revealed a significant interaction of Pitch Accent and Manipulation Step such that the more extreme L+H* phrases were rated as more black (ps<.05), though no effect was observed for the manipulation steps on the H*-containing phrases. No demographic factors, such as listener race, gender, and political affiliation, significantly affected blackness ratings.
These results reveal that listeners are sensitive to incrementally manipulated input, indicating new, promising directions for research both on listener judgments of ethnicity and in language attitudes more broadly. These findings also provide an exciting new direction for understanding the complexities of how intonation affects judgments of “sounding black” and has implications for perception, linguistic discrimination, and speech synthesis
Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) are found throughout the American West ... more Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) are found throughout the American West (Fridland et al. 2016). One CVS feature, TRAP backing, is associated with California and supposed Californian values in the popular media (Pratt and D’Onofrio 2014). It remains to be seen what regional identities or values TRAP backing indexes in communities undergoing the CVS outside of California. This study investigates the local construction of meaning for a supra-local sound change by examining how college students in Kansas, a region undergoing front lax vowel retraction, perceive TRAP backing.
Fifty-one listeners participated in a perceptual task involving stimuli read by young Kansan speakers; this task combined properties of dialect recognition tasks (Williams et al. 1999) and matched-guise tasks (Campbell-Kibler 2007). In each trial, listeners identified speakers’ regional origin and rated speakers on 14 affective Likert scales. Four critical stimuli belonged to one of two matched guises, which were acoustically manipulated such that only TRAP F2 differed between matched guises (Villarreal 2016). Conservative guises contained fronted (higher F2) TRAP and shifted guises contained backed (lower F2) TRAP.
A Bayesian analysis of regional identification found that listeners identified the shifted guise as significantly more likely to be from California than the conservative guise, indicating some association between TRAP backing and California among listeners in Kansas; guise did not affect other regional identification categories, however. A principal components analysis revealed three principal components that together accounted for 61% of the variance in ratings. PC1, measuring “general prestige,” combined seven scales, including likeable, polite, and educated; PC2, measuring “Kansan-ness,” combined two scales: Kansan and small town; and PC3, measuring “innovativeness,” combined three scales: young, feminine, and fast.
PC1 and PC3 significantly correlated with guise, with shifted guises rated higher on “general prestige” and “innovativeness” than their conservative counterparts. Conversely, PC2 significantly correlated with listeners’ regional identifications, as stimuli rated high for “Kansan-ness” were most likely to be identified as being from Kansas and least likely to be identified as being from New York or California. These results suggest that in Kansas, TRAP backing is associated with California despite local participation in the sound shift. Instead of associating TRAP backing with local identity, as Californians do, this sound change appears to index both prestige and youth in Kansas, perhaps motivating the spread of this sound change in the region. These results highlight the local construction of meaning for a sound change, while also providing some clues to the rapid supra-local spread of the CVS.
A first-wave stylistic analysis of California Vowel Shift features indicates that Californians tr... more A first-wave stylistic analysis of California Vowel Shift features indicates that Californians treat backed GOOSE as standard.
Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard (Bucholtz et al. 2007; Fought... more Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard (Bucholtz et al. 2007; Fought 2002). This view contrasts with ongoing phonological changes in California, namely the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008), which includes front lax vowels backing and lowering (e.g., [ɑ] for TRAP) and high and mid back vowels fronting (e.g., [ʉ] for GOOSE). California thus presents a situation similar to the Upper Midwest, where an ideology of standardness that locates the most “correct” English in the Upper Midwest is held so strongly that Michiganders mishear Northern Cities Shift vowels as their nonshifted counterparts when told that the speaker is a Michigander (Niedzielski 1999). Moreover, California is the subject of persistent folk-linguistic portrayals as the site of Valley girls and surfer dudes (Preston 1996).
This apparent contrast between Californians’ perceptions, sociolinguistic reality, and popular portrayals raises several questions. Do Californians recognize the CVS as Californian? What social meanings do Californians attach to the CVS? This paper addresses these questions through a dialect recognition task (Williams et al. 1999) with matched guises—California-shifted vs. conservative—differing by two representative CVS vowels: TRAP and GOOSE.
Stimuli were drawn from cartoon retell tasks conducted in sociolinguistic interviews with 12 lifelong California English speakers: three regions (San Francisco Bay Area, Lower Central Valley, Southern California) × two ethnicities (Latin@, Caucasian) × two genders (female, male). Two short excerpts (~10.5 seconds each) were drawn from each retell, and two guises were created from each excerpt by modifying F2 of each TRAP and GOOSE token via source-filter resynthesis. Californian guises featured backed TRAP (average F2: female 1626 Hz, male 1464 Hz) and fronted GOOSE (average F2: female 2119 Hz, male 1793 Hz); conservative guises featured fronted TRAP (average F2: female 1895 Hz, male 1765 Hz) and backed GOOSE (average F2: female 1640 Hz, male 1336 Hz). Ninety-seven Californians participated in the perceptual task. In each trial, listeners identified the regional origin of the speaker and rated the speaker on 12 affective scales.
A comparison of scale ratings revealed that Californian guises were rated significantly higher than conservative for sounds like a Valley girl, Californian (p < .01), confident, and rich (p < .05). In addition, significantly fewer Californian guises than conservative guises were identified as from outside California (13.5% vs. 20.8%, p < .005). These results present evidence that Californians recognize the CVS as Californian, unlike Upper Midwesterners who do not recognize the Northern Cities Shift as Upper Midwestern (Niedzielski 1999). This suggests that either the CVS is emerging as the new standard (Becker et al. in press) or Californians have a less restrictive notion of “standardness” than Upper Midwesterners (Fought 2002). Evidence from the present study suggests the latter; neither guise was rated significantly higher on the question “How suitable do you think this speaker would be for a job requiring speaking to an audience?” (p = .39).
These results suggest a connection between language variation and variation in folk-linguistic frameworks: as Californians realign their vowel system, they’re also realigning a notion of standardness.
Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard in spite of ongoing phonologi... more Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard in spite of ongoing phonological changes in California, namely the California Vowel Shift (CVS), and persistent popular folk-linguistic portrayals. This apparent contrast raises the questions of what social meanings Californians attach to the CVS, including whether they identify it as Californian. This study investigated these questions through a dialect recognition task with matched guises—California-shifted vs. conservative—differing by two representative CVS vowels (TRAP and GOOSE) manipulated via source–filter vowel resynthesis. Californian listeners heard excerpts, identified the regional origin of the speaker, and rated the speaker on 12 affective scales. Californian guises were rated significantly higher for sounds like a Valley girl, Californian, confident, and familiar, and were significantly less likely to be identified as from outside California. Results are discussed in relation to folk-linguistic awareness and possible changes in the folk-linguistic construct of “standardness.”
This paper examines the well-studied variable of coronal stop deletion (CSD) by residents of “Nor... more This paper examines the well-studied variable of coronal stop deletion (CSD) by residents of “North Town” (a pseudonym), a rural community in south Texas where Mexican Americans comprise more than 85% of the population, while Anglos comprise only 10%. Multivariate analysis of more than 3300 tokens extracted from sociolinguistic interviews with 31 Mexican Americans and 13 Anglos reveals a number of differences between constraint effects on CSD in North Town vs. other North American English varieties. As in other studies of Latin@ English, the surrounding phonological environment has the greatest effect on CSD, including among Anglos. Unlike other varieties, CSD occurs following /r/, a result that may reflect the influence of the Spanish substrate among Latin@ speakers. In contrast to Anglo varieties, past participles, regular past tense forms, and participial adjectives differ in their effects. Surprisingly, CSD occurs at a significantly higher rate in Anglo than in Mexican American speech.
Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, th... more Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, this project addresses tensions that emerge in the teaching of World Englishes at the undergraduate level. The presenters analyze how students understand and verbalize issues in current World Englishes debates and become better listeners of English dialects.
Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, th... more Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, this project addresses tensions that emerge in the teaching of World Englishes at the undergraduate level. The presenters analyze how students understand and verbalize issues in current World Englishes debates and become better listeners of English dialects.
A dialect recognition task (e.g., Williams, Garrett, & Coupland 1999) was conducted in which Cali... more A dialect recognition task (e.g., Williams, Garrett, & Coupland 1999) was conducted in which Californians listened to speakers from different regions of the state (Far Northern California, Bay Area, Lower Central Valley, Southern California), guessed speakers’ regional origin, and rated speakers on language attitudes scales. The data revealed that the most populous regions of the state enjoy prestige, with the Bay Area perceived as having higher status and Southern California greater solidarity. Listeners from both regions believed their own region to be central to a notion of California speech, while placing rurality at the periphery of what it means to sound Californian.
This study provides a greater understanding of the process of developing identity in a second lan... more This study provides a greater understanding of the process of developing identity in a second language by examining L2 Spanish learners’ use of informal and formal address forms (tú and usted). Specifically, this study sought to determine how Spanish learners conceive of their use of the address form system, how these same learners actually use address forms in real time, to what extent their metapragmatic judgments match their actual production, and what the implications of this match or mismatch are for their nascent L2 social identities. Twelve native English-speaking intermediate-level Spanish learners interacted with Spanish speakers in oral role-play scenarios designed to elicit address forms, then answered a questionnaire one week later about desired address form use in scenarios that were matched with role-play scenarios. Learners exhibited an overgeneralization of the informal tú in both judgments and (especially) production, a result that stands in stark contrast to French learners’ overgeneralization of the formal usted in particular (Kinginger & Farrell 2004) and formal sociolinguistic variants in general (Howard et al. in press). A qualitative analysis of questionnaire judgments revealed that—contrary to views of pre-study-abroad learners as sociolinguistically naïve—these intermediate learners were both cognizant of the second-order indexical potential of address forms (Morford 1997) and determined to utilize this indexical potential in attempting to construct an L2 social identity. A logistic regression analysis of 614 address-form tokens from role-play interactions further revealed that while pronoun type significantly affected address form choice, a learner’s judgment for a given scenario was the strongest significant predictor of address form choice. Whereas some learners failed to consistently produce their desired address forms, others showed an adeptness at matching their production to their judgments. I thus discuss the notion of agency in accounting for individual learners’ ability to fulfill a self-constructed L2 social identity.
Papers by Dan Villarreal
Penn Working Papers in Linguistics, 2018
Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of the Phonetic Sciences, 2019
This experiment assessed the validity of a statistical classification method for automated coding... more This experiment assessed the validity of a statistical classification method for automated coding of sociophonetic variables, in particular the presence vs. absence of English non-prevocalic /r/. A random forest classifier was trained on 180 acoustic measures from 5,355 tokens of /r/ (hand-coded as Present or Absent) in a variety of English with variable rhoticity; the classifier achieved 87.9% accuracy on training data. The classifier was then used to predict the probability of /r/ presence in 32,099 additional tokens from the same variety.
Eleven phonetically trained listeners judged 60 classifier-coded tokens as Present or Absent. Judgment results indicated a significant positive linear relationship between classifier probability and human judgments; classifier probability also outperformed individual acoustic measures (e.g., F3 minimum) in predicting human judgments. These results both validate this random forest classifier method for automated coding of sociophonetic variables and indicate the viability of modelling phonetic variation using classifier probability.
New Zealand Linguistic Society 2019, 2019
New Zealand Linguistic Society, 2018
Southland English has historically been New Zealand’s only (partially) rhotic variety. There has ... more Southland English has historically been New Zealand’s only (partially) rhotic variety. There has only been one large-scale study of Southland (r), which suggested a resurgence of rhoticity following NURSE among young women (Bartlett 2002). We build on this work, using modern statistical methods to better understand the linguistic and social conditioning of change in Southland (r).
We analysed over 20,000 tokens of non-prevocalic Southland (r), coded as present/absent. 20% of tokens were hand-coded; the rest were automatically coded via a random-forest classifier trained on the hand-coded tokens to predict (r) presence/absence based on 180 acoustic measures (this auto-coder achieved over 80% accuracy on the hand-coded training set). Data were modelled via logistic mixed-effects regression, with a three-way generation distinction (birth years 1900–30, 1931–55, 1956–80).
As expected, this analysis reveals a significant effect for vowel, with greater rhoticity for NURSE than other vowels, and a significant effect for generation, which indicates a change in apparent time. The statistical modelling allows us to see further fine-grained phonological and grammatical conditioning of the change as it progressed throughout the speech community. For instance, we find that NURSE before non-sibilant fricatives lags in rhoticity among the oldest speakers but catches up to other NURSE environments among middle and young speakers. We also find that the increase of rhoticity in NURSE appeared first in content words, then spread to function words. We discuss the full trajectory of change for Southland (r) and highlight some implications for theories of phonological change more generally.
Experimental studies of priming have shown that we have a strong tendency to repeat linguistic ma... more Experimental studies of priming have shown that we have a strong tendency to repeat linguistic material that we have recently produced, seen or heard (Bock 1986; Bock & Branigan et al 2000). A growing body of work from corpus linguistics and sociolinguistics suggests that this also happens in more natural types of speech (Gries 2005; Szmrecsanyi 2006; Poplack 1980; Travis 2007). The idea that speakers use clusters of similar variants in natural running speech has been reported before in sociolinguistics but the suggestion has been that repetition is under the control of the speaker as they engage in the construction and maintenance of sociolinguistic style (Eckert and Rickford 2001). This paper is part of a recent attempt to open up an intriguing possibility: that previous work invoking style as an explanation could perhaps be equally well, if not better, explained by models of priming (Tamminga (2016), Clark (2014, 2018).
This paper examines evidence for within-speaker repetition effects by exploring data from a corpus of NZ English monologues. We present data from 4 phonological variables: word medial intervocalic /t/, KIT, DRESS and STRUT. By fitting mixed effect regression models incorporating both the realisation of the preceding variable, and a range of other linguistic and social predictors, we show two main findings that are of interest to sociolinguistics:
• Speakers show a repetition effect reminiscent of priming for those variables undergoing change (i.e. medial /t/, KIT and DRESS). This has methodological implications: it could mean that a proportion of the variation that has previously been attributed to sociolinguistic factors could potentially be attributed to more mechanistic repetition effects.
• In the case of medial /t/, priming interacts with the speaker’s gender. No psycholinguistic work predicts different priming behaviour for males and females and most sociolinguistic work assumes that men and women from the same speech community will share the same ‘internal’ constraints on variation.
We therefore argue that this work benefits sociolinguistics by suggesting that phonological variation is responsive not only to style but also to psycholinguistic factors like priming.
Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) have been found throughout the American... more Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) have been found throughout the American West (Fridland et al. 2016). One CVS feature, TRAP backing, is associated with California and Californian values (Villarreal 2016). It remains to be seen what regional identities or values TRAP backing indexes in communities undergoing the CVS outside of California. This study investigates the local construction of meaning for a supra-local sound change by examining how listeners in the Midwestern state of Kansas, which is undergoing front lax vowel retraction, perceive TRAP backing.
Fifty-one university students participated in a matched-guise task featuring stimuli read by young Kansan speakers. In each trial, listeners rated speakers on 14 Likert scales. Four critical stimuli belonged to one of two matched guises, which were acoustically manipulated such that only TRAP F2 differed between matched guises. Conservative guises contained fronted TRAP and shifted guises contained backed TRAP.
A principal components analysis of ratings revealed three principal components: PC1, measuring “general prestige,” PC2, measuring “Kansan-ness,” and PC3, measuring “innovativeness.” PC1 and PC3 significantly correlated with guise (p < .05), with shifted guises rated higher on “general prestige” and “innovativeness.” Conversely, PC2 significantly correlated with listeners’ regional identifications (p < .001), as stimuli rated high for “Kansan-ness” were most likely to be identified as being from Kansas and least likely to be identified as being from New York or California.
These results suggest that instead of associating TRAP backing with local identity, as in California, this sound change appears to index prestige and youth in Kansas, perhaps motivating the spread of this sound change in the region. These results provide clues to the rapid spread of the CVS, highlighting the local construction of meaning for a supra-local sound change.
This matched-guise experiment assessed whether sociolinguistic judgments are subject to increment... more This matched-guise experiment assessed whether sociolinguistic judgments are subject to incrementality, with judgments increasing in magnitude as variable stimuli demonstrate more extreme differences in some critical dimension. In particular, this task tested whether it is possible to trigger judgments of Barack Obama as “more” or “less” black (e.g., Alim & Smitherman 2011) by manipulating his intonational contours.
Stimuli were derived from twenty declarative Intonational Phrases with H* and/or L+H* pitch accents (following ToBI conventions for Mainstream American English of Beckman and Ayers 1997). Ten phrases featured L+H* pitch accents—which occur more frequency in African American Language (AAL) (Holliday 2016)—and ten featured H* pitch accents. Four stimuli apiece were created from these twenty phrases by using Praat to make each pitch accent more extreme by semitone-based F0 steps, yielding a four-way Manipulation Step factor. Seventy-nine American English listeners rated these stimuli in randomized order by responding to the question “How black does Obama sound here?”.
A mixed-effects regression model of ratings assessed the interaction of Pitch Accent type, Manipulation Step, and Boundary Tone, with Phrase nested within Pitch Accent. No main effect for Pitch Accent type emerged, indicating no rating differences for L+H* and H* phrases overall (p=.27), despite the higher likelihood of occurrence of L+H* in AAL. However, the interaction of Pitch Accent and Manipulation Step was significant, with more extreme L+H* phrases rated as more black (ps<.05); no such effect was observed among manipulation steps for H*-containing phrases. No listener demographic factors (e.g., listener race, gender, political affiliation) significantly affected blackness ratings.
These results indicate that listeners’ sociolinguistic perceptions are sensitive to the magnitude of the input, a finding that indicates promising directions for research in language attitudes and sociolinguistic cognition.
Suprasegmental features, especially intonation and voice quality, may be of special interest to s... more Suprasegmental features, especially intonation and voice quality, may be of special interest to sociolinguists as well as the public, due to their high level of salience for listeners (Purnell et al. 1999, Baugh 2015, Thomas 2015, Holliday 2016). Nevertheless, suprasegmentals have received little in the way of perceptual study. Building on the findings of Alim and Smitherman (2011), who assert that Barack Obama often employs a unique intonational style that appeals to both white and black audiences, this matched-guise experiment tested whether it is possible to trigger judgments of Obama as “more” or “less” black by manipulating aspects of his intonation. Moreover, this experiment assessed whether sociolinguistic judgments are subject to incrementality, with judgments increasing in magnitude as the variable stimuli demonstrate more extreme differences in F0.
This study builds on as well as intonational methodologies by testing listener judgments of manipulations of Obama’s utterances in one interview. The utterances were declarative Intonational Phrases with H* and/or L+H* pitch accents (following ToBI conventions for Mainstream American English of Beckman and Ayers 1997). Ten phrases featured L+H* pitch accents—which occur more frequency in AAL (McLarty 2011, Thomas 2015, Holliday 2016)— and ten featured H* pitch accents. Four critical stimuli apiece were created from these twenty phrases by using Praat to make each pitch accent more extreme by F0 steps (i.e., H* accents were raised at maximum F0 by one-semitone steps, and L+H* accents were raised at maximum F0 by one-semitone steps and lowered at minimum F0 by half-semitone steps), leading to a fourway Manipulation Step factor. Seventy-nine American English listeners heard these 80 critical stimuli, with 40 fillers, in randomized order in an online experiment. For each trial, listeners responded to the question “How black does Obama sound here?” using a 100-point slider bar.
A mixed-effects regression model of normalized ratings assessed the interaction of Pitch Accent type, Manipulation Step, and Boundary Tone, with Phrase nested within Pitch Accent. No main effect for Pitch Accent type was found, indicating that, overall, L+H* phrases are not rated more or less black than H* phrases (p=.27), despite the higher likelihood of occurrence of L+H* in AAL. However, the model revealed a significant interaction of Pitch Accent and Manipulation Step such that the more extreme L+H* phrases were rated as more black (ps<.05), though no effect was observed for the manipulation steps on the H*-containing phrases. No demographic factors, such as listener race, gender, and political affiliation, significantly affected blackness ratings.
These results reveal that listeners are sensitive to incrementally manipulated input, indicating new, promising directions for research both on listener judgments of ethnicity and in language attitudes more broadly. These findings also provide an exciting new direction for understanding the complexities of how intonation affects judgments of “sounding black” and has implications for perception, linguistic discrimination, and speech synthesis
Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) are found throughout the American West ... more Features of the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008) are found throughout the American West (Fridland et al. 2016). One CVS feature, TRAP backing, is associated with California and supposed Californian values in the popular media (Pratt and D’Onofrio 2014). It remains to be seen what regional identities or values TRAP backing indexes in communities undergoing the CVS outside of California. This study investigates the local construction of meaning for a supra-local sound change by examining how college students in Kansas, a region undergoing front lax vowel retraction, perceive TRAP backing.
Fifty-one listeners participated in a perceptual task involving stimuli read by young Kansan speakers; this task combined properties of dialect recognition tasks (Williams et al. 1999) and matched-guise tasks (Campbell-Kibler 2007). In each trial, listeners identified speakers’ regional origin and rated speakers on 14 affective Likert scales. Four critical stimuli belonged to one of two matched guises, which were acoustically manipulated such that only TRAP F2 differed between matched guises (Villarreal 2016). Conservative guises contained fronted (higher F2) TRAP and shifted guises contained backed (lower F2) TRAP.
A Bayesian analysis of regional identification found that listeners identified the shifted guise as significantly more likely to be from California than the conservative guise, indicating some association between TRAP backing and California among listeners in Kansas; guise did not affect other regional identification categories, however. A principal components analysis revealed three principal components that together accounted for 61% of the variance in ratings. PC1, measuring “general prestige,” combined seven scales, including likeable, polite, and educated; PC2, measuring “Kansan-ness,” combined two scales: Kansan and small town; and PC3, measuring “innovativeness,” combined three scales: young, feminine, and fast.
PC1 and PC3 significantly correlated with guise, with shifted guises rated higher on “general prestige” and “innovativeness” than their conservative counterparts. Conversely, PC2 significantly correlated with listeners’ regional identifications, as stimuli rated high for “Kansan-ness” were most likely to be identified as being from Kansas and least likely to be identified as being from New York or California. These results suggest that in Kansas, TRAP backing is associated with California despite local participation in the sound shift. Instead of associating TRAP backing with local identity, as Californians do, this sound change appears to index both prestige and youth in Kansas, perhaps motivating the spread of this sound change in the region. These results highlight the local construction of meaning for a sound change, while also providing some clues to the rapid supra-local spread of the CVS.
A first-wave stylistic analysis of California Vowel Shift features indicates that Californians tr... more A first-wave stylistic analysis of California Vowel Shift features indicates that Californians treat backed GOOSE as standard.
Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard (Bucholtz et al. 2007; Fought... more Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard (Bucholtz et al. 2007; Fought 2002). This view contrasts with ongoing phonological changes in California, namely the California Vowel Shift (CVS; Eckert 2008), which includes front lax vowels backing and lowering (e.g., [ɑ] for TRAP) and high and mid back vowels fronting (e.g., [ʉ] for GOOSE). California thus presents a situation similar to the Upper Midwest, where an ideology of standardness that locates the most “correct” English in the Upper Midwest is held so strongly that Michiganders mishear Northern Cities Shift vowels as their nonshifted counterparts when told that the speaker is a Michigander (Niedzielski 1999). Moreover, California is the subject of persistent folk-linguistic portrayals as the site of Valley girls and surfer dudes (Preston 1996).
This apparent contrast between Californians’ perceptions, sociolinguistic reality, and popular portrayals raises several questions. Do Californians recognize the CVS as Californian? What social meanings do Californians attach to the CVS? This paper addresses these questions through a dialect recognition task (Williams et al. 1999) with matched guises—California-shifted vs. conservative—differing by two representative CVS vowels: TRAP and GOOSE.
Stimuli were drawn from cartoon retell tasks conducted in sociolinguistic interviews with 12 lifelong California English speakers: three regions (San Francisco Bay Area, Lower Central Valley, Southern California) × two ethnicities (Latin@, Caucasian) × two genders (female, male). Two short excerpts (~10.5 seconds each) were drawn from each retell, and two guises were created from each excerpt by modifying F2 of each TRAP and GOOSE token via source-filter resynthesis. Californian guises featured backed TRAP (average F2: female 1626 Hz, male 1464 Hz) and fronted GOOSE (average F2: female 2119 Hz, male 1793 Hz); conservative guises featured fronted TRAP (average F2: female 1895 Hz, male 1765 Hz) and backed GOOSE (average F2: female 1640 Hz, male 1336 Hz). Ninety-seven Californians participated in the perceptual task. In each trial, listeners identified the regional origin of the speaker and rated the speaker on 12 affective scales.
A comparison of scale ratings revealed that Californian guises were rated significantly higher than conservative for sounds like a Valley girl, Californian (p < .01), confident, and rich (p < .05). In addition, significantly fewer Californian guises than conservative guises were identified as from outside California (13.5% vs. 20.8%, p < .005). These results present evidence that Californians recognize the CVS as Californian, unlike Upper Midwesterners who do not recognize the Northern Cities Shift as Upper Midwestern (Niedzielski 1999). This suggests that either the CVS is emerging as the new standard (Becker et al. in press) or Californians have a less restrictive notion of “standardness” than Upper Midwesterners (Fought 2002). Evidence from the present study suggests the latter; neither guise was rated significantly higher on the question “How suitable do you think this speaker would be for a job requiring speaking to an audience?” (p = .39).
These results suggest a connection between language variation and variation in folk-linguistic frameworks: as Californians realign their vowel system, they’re also realigning a notion of standardness.
Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard in spite of ongoing phonologi... more Californians tend to view California English as relatively standard in spite of ongoing phonological changes in California, namely the California Vowel Shift (CVS), and persistent popular folk-linguistic portrayals. This apparent contrast raises the questions of what social meanings Californians attach to the CVS, including whether they identify it as Californian. This study investigated these questions through a dialect recognition task with matched guises—California-shifted vs. conservative—differing by two representative CVS vowels (TRAP and GOOSE) manipulated via source–filter vowel resynthesis. Californian listeners heard excerpts, identified the regional origin of the speaker, and rated the speaker on 12 affective scales. Californian guises were rated significantly higher for sounds like a Valley girl, Californian, confident, and familiar, and were significantly less likely to be identified as from outside California. Results are discussed in relation to folk-linguistic awareness and possible changes in the folk-linguistic construct of “standardness.”
This paper examines the well-studied variable of coronal stop deletion (CSD) by residents of “Nor... more This paper examines the well-studied variable of coronal stop deletion (CSD) by residents of “North Town” (a pseudonym), a rural community in south Texas where Mexican Americans comprise more than 85% of the population, while Anglos comprise only 10%. Multivariate analysis of more than 3300 tokens extracted from sociolinguistic interviews with 31 Mexican Americans and 13 Anglos reveals a number of differences between constraint effects on CSD in North Town vs. other North American English varieties. As in other studies of Latin@ English, the surrounding phonological environment has the greatest effect on CSD, including among Anglos. Unlike other varieties, CSD occurs following /r/, a result that may reflect the influence of the Spanish substrate among Latin@ speakers. In contrast to Anglo varieties, past participles, regular past tense forms, and participial adjectives differ in their effects. Surprisingly, CSD occurs at a significantly higher rate in Anglo than in Mexican American speech.
Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, th... more Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, this project addresses tensions that emerge in the teaching of World Englishes at the undergraduate level. The presenters analyze how students understand and verbalize issues in current World Englishes debates and become better listeners of English dialects.
Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, th... more Drawing on existing research and current local and global rationales about internationalizing, this project addresses tensions that emerge in the teaching of World Englishes at the undergraduate level. The presenters analyze how students understand and verbalize issues in current World Englishes debates and become better listeners of English dialects.
A dialect recognition task (e.g., Williams, Garrett, & Coupland 1999) was conducted in which Cali... more A dialect recognition task (e.g., Williams, Garrett, & Coupland 1999) was conducted in which Californians listened to speakers from different regions of the state (Far Northern California, Bay Area, Lower Central Valley, Southern California), guessed speakers’ regional origin, and rated speakers on language attitudes scales. The data revealed that the most populous regions of the state enjoy prestige, with the Bay Area perceived as having higher status and Southern California greater solidarity. Listeners from both regions believed their own region to be central to a notion of California speech, while placing rurality at the periphery of what it means to sound Californian.
This study provides a greater understanding of the process of developing identity in a second lan... more This study provides a greater understanding of the process of developing identity in a second language by examining L2 Spanish learners’ use of informal and formal address forms (tú and usted). Specifically, this study sought to determine how Spanish learners conceive of their use of the address form system, how these same learners actually use address forms in real time, to what extent their metapragmatic judgments match their actual production, and what the implications of this match or mismatch are for their nascent L2 social identities. Twelve native English-speaking intermediate-level Spanish learners interacted with Spanish speakers in oral role-play scenarios designed to elicit address forms, then answered a questionnaire one week later about desired address form use in scenarios that were matched with role-play scenarios. Learners exhibited an overgeneralization of the informal tú in both judgments and (especially) production, a result that stands in stark contrast to French learners’ overgeneralization of the formal usted in particular (Kinginger & Farrell 2004) and formal sociolinguistic variants in general (Howard et al. in press). A qualitative analysis of questionnaire judgments revealed that—contrary to views of pre-study-abroad learners as sociolinguistically naïve—these intermediate learners were both cognizant of the second-order indexical potential of address forms (Morford 1997) and determined to utilize this indexical potential in attempting to construct an L2 social identity. A logistic regression analysis of 614 address-form tokens from role-play interactions further revealed that while pronoun type significantly affected address form choice, a learner’s judgment for a given scenario was the strongest significant predictor of address form choice. Whereas some learners failed to consistently produce their desired address forms, others showed an adeptness at matching their production to their judgments. I thus discuss the notion of agency in accounting for individual learners’ ability to fulfill a self-constructed L2 social identity.
Penn Working Papers in Linguistics, 2018
Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of the Phonetic Sciences, 2019
This experiment assessed the validity of a statistical classification method for automated coding... more This experiment assessed the validity of a statistical classification method for automated coding of sociophonetic variables, in particular the presence vs. absence of English non-prevocalic /r/. A random forest classifier was trained on 180 acoustic measures from 5,355 tokens of /r/ (hand-coded as Present or Absent) in a variety of English with variable rhoticity; the classifier achieved 87.9% accuracy on training data. The classifier was then used to predict the probability of /r/ presence in 32,099 additional tokens from the same variety.
Eleven phonetically trained listeners judged 60 classifier-coded tokens as Present or Absent. Judgment results indicated a significant positive linear relationship between classifier probability and human judgments; classifier probability also outperformed individual acoustic measures (e.g., F3 minimum) in predicting human judgments. These results both validate this random forest classifier method for automated coding of sociophonetic variables and indicate the viability of modelling phonetic variation using classifier probability.
Journal of English Linguistics, 2018
Past production research on the California Vowel Shift (CVS) has suggested that the CVS carries s... more Past production research on the California Vowel Shift (CVS) has suggested that the CVS carries social meanings of carefreeness, Whiteness, femininity, and privilege (e.g., Eckert 2008b), but it is unclear whether these social meanings reflect listener perceptions. In the present study, Californian listeners heard speech samples, guessed where speakers were from, and rated speakers on language attitudes scales; stimuli in this task were matched guises differing by speakers' use of two CVS features. The results indicated that listeners associate these features with Californianness, sounding like a Valley girl, and (for male speakers) confidence, complicating the social meanings suggested by production studies. I discuss these results in terms of how interaction context guides the perception of social meaning by activating subsets of the indexical field. This research also introduces two innovative methods for investigating sociolinguistic perception: stimuli created using resynthesized vowels within spontaneous speech produced by multiple speakers, and statistical inference via Bayesian hierarchical modeling.
Past production research on the California Vowel Shift (CVS) has suggested that the CVS carries s... more Past production research on the California Vowel Shift (CVS) has suggested that the CVS carries social meanings of carefreeness, Whiteness, femininity, and privilege (e.g., Eckert 2008b), but it is unclear whether these social meanings reflect listener perceptions. Californian listeners heard speech samples, guessed where speakers were from, and rated speakers on language attitudes scales; stimuli in this task were matched guises differing by speakers' use of two CVS features. The results indicated that listeners associate these features with Californian-ness, sounding like a Valley girl, and (for male speakers) confidence, complicating the social meanings suggested by production studies. I discuss these results in terms of how interaction context guides the perception of social meaning by activating subsets of the indexical field. This research also introduces two innovative methods for investigating sociolinguistic perception: stimuli created using resynthesized vowels within spontaneous speech produced by multiple speakers, and statistical inference via Bayesian hierarchical modeling.
Although Californian ways of speaking have long been an object of folk-linguistic awareness and s... more Although Californian ways of speaking have long been an object of folk-linguistic awareness and stereotyping by non-Californian speakers (e.g., Preston 1996) and in the media (e.g., Frank Zappa’s “Valley Girl” and Saturday Night Live’s “The Californians”), relatively little is known about what Californians hear when they hear one another (Bucholtz et al. 2007). A dialect recognition task (e.g., Williams, Garrett, and Coupland 1999) was conducted in which listeners heard samples of speakers from different regions of the state (Far Northern California, Bay Area, Lower Central Valley, Southern California) to guess where speakers were from. Listeners also rated speakers on language attitudes scales (likeable, educated, Californian, etc.). A comparison of regional identification and scale ratings revealed that the most populous regions of the state enjoy favorable attitudes, with the San Francisco Bay Area perceived to have greater correctness and Southern California greater pleasantness. Listeners from both of these regions believed their own region to be central to a notion of California speech, placing Far Northern California, the Lower Central Valley, and rurality in general at the periphery of what it means to sound Californian.
Research on social meaning, which links language variation to the wider social world, often bases... more Research on social meaning, which links language variation to the wider social world, often bases claims about the social meanings of linguistic forms on production (i.e., speakers' situational use of meaningful forms). In the case of the California Vowel Shift (CVS), an ongoing restructuring of the vowel system of California English that takes place below the level of conscious awareness, previous production research has suggested that the CVS carries social meanings of carefreeness, femininity, and privilege. Left unclear in these production-based claims is whether listeners actually pick up on and recognize the social meanings that speakers apparently utilize the CVS to transmit. In this research, a dialect recognition task with matched guises (California-shifted vs. conservative) forms the basis for exploring Californian listeners' reactions to the CVS, and how these reactions are mediated by perceptions of dialect geography. In short, this research focuses on listeners' reactions to the CVS in order to address a more fundamental question: How do listeners and speakers together participate in the construction of social meaning? Stimuli for the main study task were drawn from excerpts of sociolinguistic interviews with 12 lifelong California English speakers from three regions of the state: the San Francisco Bay Area, Lower Central Valley, and Southern California. Guises were created from interview excerpts by modifying the F2 of each TRAP and GOOSE token via source-filter resynthesis methods. Californian guises featured backed TRAP and fronted GOOSE; conservative guises featured fronted
CATESOL Journal, 2013
This mixed-method study addressed the “foreign TA problem,” reconceptualizing it as the communica... more This mixed-method study addressed the “foreign TA problem,” reconceptualizing it as the communication gap, an issue created by multiple parties—including bias originating from undergraduates. Experimental sessions measured undergraduates’ comprehension of two brief lessons taught by an international professor; in between lessons, participants completed one of three short modules: a bias-stimulation module, a control module, or a linguistic-training module (which confronted both accent misunderstanding and accent bias). While training did not affect comprehension, questionnaire responses revealed a positive effect on sociolinguistic attitudes. Follow-up discussion sessions explored undergraduates’ experiences with international faculty and responses to the communication gap. Several important themes emerged from these discussions, including effects on academic plans, negative cognitive effects, and a model of undergraduates’ socialization into accent bias. The article concludes with recommendations specifically geared toward TESOL professionals’ ongoing efforts to close the communication gap, including a greater recognition of undergraduates’ role in perpetuating the communication gap.
Journal of Pragmatics, May 2014
This study investigates the L2 identities of native English-speaking intermediate-level Spanish l... more This study investigates the L2 identities of native English-speaking intermediate-level Spanish learners by combining two methods, production and metapragmatic judgments, and focusing on T/V address forms (tú and usted). Twelve intermediate-level Spanish learners interacted with Spanish speakers in oral role-play scenarios designed to elicit different address forms. Learners answered a questionnaire one week later about desired address form use in various interactional scenarios (which were matched with role-play scenarios). An analysis of questionnaire judgments revealed that these learners were both cognizant of the second-order indexical potential of T/V address forms and determined to utilize this indexical potential to construct notions of their desired L2 identities. Conversely, learners’ address-form production exhibited an overgeneralization of T, a result that starkly contrasts with the overgeneralization of V found in previous studies. Whereas some learners failed to consistently produce their desired address forms, compromising their selfconstructed ideal L2 identities, others showed an adeptness at matching their production to their judgments. I thus suggest that future research on L2 pragmatics and L2 identity move beyond a focus on L2 pragmatic competence and toward a consideration of learners’ indexical awareness and agency.