Development of consciousness - The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development (original) (raw)

References

Bråten, S. (2009). The intersubjective mirror in infant learning and evolution of speech. Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2012). The phenomenological mind (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2004). Brain development. In Gregory, R.L. (Ed.), Oxford companion to the mind (2nd ed., pp. 116–127). Oxford, UK: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2004). Infancy, mind in. In Gregory, R.L. (Ed.), Oxford companion to the mind (2nd ed., pp. 455–464). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Whitehead, A.N. (1926). Science and the modern world. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Bruner, J.S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Bullowa, M. (Ed.) (1979). Before speech: The beginning of human communication. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar

Delafield-Butt, J.T., & Gangopadhyay, N. (2013). Sensorimotor intentionality: The origins of intentionality in prospective agent action. Developmental Review, 33, 399–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Delafield-Butt, J.T., & Trevarthen, C. (2015). The ontogenesis of narrative: From moving to meaning. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Donaldson, M. (1992). Human minds: An exploration. London, UK: Allen Lane.Google Scholar

Goodrich, B.G. (2010). We do, therefore we think: Time, motility, and consciousness. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 21, 331–361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Kugiumutzakis, G., & Trevarthen, C. (2015). Neonatal imitation. In Wright, J.D. (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (Vol. 16, 2nd ed., pp. 481–488). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar

Malloch, S., & Trevarthen, C. (Eds.) (2009). Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Merker, B. (2013) The efference cascade, consciousness, and its self: Naturalizing the first person pivot of action control. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T.M. & Fox, N.A. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar

Packard, A., & Delafield-Butt, J.T. (2014). Feelings as agents of selection: Putting Charles Darwin back into (extended neo-) Darwinism. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 112, 332–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Panksepp, J., & Biven, L. (2012). Archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar

Piontelli, A. (1992). From fetus to child. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar

Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Reissland, N., & Kisilevsky, B. (Eds.) (2015). Fetal development: Research on brain and behavior, environmental influences, and emerging technologies. London, UK: Springer.Google Scholar

Sherrington, C.S. (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Stern, D.N. (2010). Forms of vitality: Exploring dynamic experience in psychology, the arts, psychotherapy and development. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2001). Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: Their origin, development and significance for infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 95–131.Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2009). The functions of emotion in infancy: The regulation and communication of rhythm, sympathy, and meaning in human development. In Fosha, D., Siegel, D.J., & Solomon, M.F. (Eds.), The healing power of emotion: Affective neuroscience, development, and clinical practice (pp. 55–85). New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2015) Infant semiosis: The psycho-biology of action and shared experience from birth. Cognitive Development, 36, 130–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2016). From the intrinsic motive pulse of infant actions, to the life time of cultural meanings. In Mölder, B., Arstila, V., & Ohrstrom, P. (Eds.), Philosophy and psychology of time, Springer studies in brain and mind (Vol. 9, pp. 225–265). Dordrecht, NL: Springer.Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C., & Delafield-Butt, J.T. (2013). Biology of shared meaning and language development: Regulating the life of narratives. In Legerstee, M., Haley, D., & Bornstein, M. (Eds.), The infant mind: Origins of the social brain (pp. 167–199). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C., & Panksepp, J. (2016). In tune with feeling: Musical play with emotions of creativity, inspiring neuroaffective development and self-confidence for learning in company. In Hart, S. (Ed.), Inclusion, play and empathy: Neuroaffective development in children’s groups (pp. 29–54). London, UK: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C., & Reddy, V. (2007). Consciousness in infants. In Velmans, M. & Schneider, S. (Eds.), A companion to consciousness (pp. 41–57). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C., Aitken, K.J., Vandekerckhove, M., Delafield-Butt, J., & Nagy, E. (2006). Collaborative regulations of vitality in early childhood: Stress in intimate relationships and postnatal psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D.J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2, 2nd ed., pp. 65–126). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar

Zoia, S., Blason, L., D’Ottavio, G., Bulgheroni, M., Pezzetta, E., Scabar, A., & Castiello, U. (2007). Evidence of early development of action planning in the human foetus: A kinematic study. Experimental Brain Research, 176, 217–226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed