THE SELECTIVE ADVANTAGE OF CRYPSIS IN MICE (original) (raw)

Journal Article

,

Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology and The Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, 26 Oxford St, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology and The Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, 26 Oxford St, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138

Search for other works by this author on:

Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology and The Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, 26 Oxford St, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138

Search for other works by this author on:

Received:

07 October 2009

Accepted:

29 January 2010

Navbar Search Filter Mobile Enter search term Search

Abstract

The light color of mice that inhabit the sandy dunes of Florida's coast have served as a textbook example of adaptation for nearly a century, despite the fact that the selective advantage of crypsis has never been directly tested or quantified in nature. Using plasticine mouse models of light and dark color, we demonstrate a strong selective advantage for mice that match their local background substrate. Further our data suggest that stabilizing selection maintains color matching within a single habitat, as models that are both lighter and darker than their local environment are selected against. These results provide empirical evidence in support of the hypothesis that visual hunting predators shape color patterning in Peromyscus mice and suggest a mechanism by which selection drives the pronounced color variation among populations.

One-hundred fifty years ago in The Origin of Species, Charles Darwin famously proposed his theory of evolution by natural selection. To build the argument for natural selection, Darwin often relied on intuitive examples: “when we see…the alpine ptarmigan white in winter, the red grouse the color of heather, and the black-grouse of peaty earth, we must believe that these tints are of service … in preserving them from danger” (Darwin 1859). Perhaps inspired by Darwin's intuition that camouflage confers a survival advantage, color variation became the focus of many of the early studies of adaptation. In particular, studies on color matching in Peromyscus mice were instrumental in documenting natural selection in the wild—strong correlations between local soil color and dorsal coat color were repeatedly found among populations (e.g., Dice 1940). Further, JBS Haldane (1948) demonstrated theoretically that spatially varying selection could lead to and maintain locally adapted phenotypes, again using Peromyscus color matching as the prime empirical example. These few early studies were broadly seen as evidence that selection could drive color matching in a variety of taxa.

Despite nearly a century of work on this system, however, a direct and empirical quantification of the selective advantage for color matching in a natural setting is still lacking. Enclosure experiments have been suggestive, albeit contrived—these studies often use unnatural densities of mice, extreme color variants, and/or largely depend on the personalities of one or two individual predators (“the long horned owl was very shy and erratic in his behavior,” whereas “the barn owl was a much more consistent worker”; Dice 1949). Moreover, even with enclosure experiments and more traditional common-garden approaches, it is difficult to disentangle direct selection on color (due to crypsis) from selection on other traits (e.g., odor, activity level, or escape behavior) that may be correlated with color. To address these concerns, recent research has taken a novel experimental approach—using photographs (Webster et al. 2009), manipulated prey (Cuthill et al. 2005; Ioannou and Krause 2009), and computer-generated images (Kiltie and Laine 1992; Chlao et al. 2007)—to empirically test hypotheses about the adaptive significance and function of animal color (Stevens and Merilaita 2009). Similar to these studies, here we expose predators to prey models to directly estimate the selective advantage of camouflage in nature.

The use of plasticine models to address questions in evolutionary biology was pioneered by Brodie (1993) and since has been used to document predation in reptile and amphibian species. These studies have demonstrated how variation in aposematism (e.g., Kuchta 2005; Saporito et al. 2007; Noonan and Comeault 2009), Batesian mimicry (e.g., Brodie and Janzen 1995; Pfennig et al. 2001), body shape (e.g., Shepard 2007), and sexual signaling (e.g., Husak et al. 2006) affect predation rates. Although simple, this method has several advantages. First, because plasticine preserves evidence of predation attempts (i.e., tooth, beak or claw imprints), it is possible to quantify both predation rate and predator type. Moreover, using models, we can deploy a large, and biologically realistic, number of individuals within a given environment. Finally, this experimental approach allows us to focus on variation in a single trait of interest (and thereby control for indirect selection on correlated traits) because models can be made otherwise identical. Here, we use plasticine models of mice, which differ in only color phenotype, to test for and quantify the selective advantage of camouflaging color in realistic models by exposing them to natural predators in the wild.

Materials and Methods

The most extreme case of color adaptation in Peromyscus occurs among subspecies of _Peromyscus polionotus_—the dark-brown dorsal coats of inland subspecies occupying abandoned agricultural fields (oldfields) contrast strikingly with the pale-colored beach mice that inhabit Florida's coastal sand dunes and barrier islands (Sumner 1929a,b). Using nonhardening plasticine, we constructed 250 models of P. polionotus, half of which were painted to mimic the coat color and pattern of the dark oldfield mouse (Peromyscus polionotus subgriseus) and half the light Santa Rosa Island beach mouse (P. p. leucocephalus). We then deployed these models in both light and dark habitat, recorded predation events, and estimated the selective advantage of camouflage.

MOUSE MODEL CONSTRUCTION

To produce the most realistic models, we created silicone molds of P. polionotus specimens preserved in a crouched position and poured fully liquefied Van Aken Plastilina into these molds. When cooled, we removed models and painted them using Rustoleum (Rust-Oleum, Vernon Hills, IL) textured spray paint. All models were first spray-coated with white, the dark models received an additional coat of gray then tan paint, and both types were finalized with hand painting of eyes and dorsal pelage in either brown (dark models) or tan (light models).

MODEL DEPLOYMENT

Models were simultaneously deployed in eight linear transects, four set in beach habitat, at Topsail Hill Preserve State Park (N30°21′50.831″W86°17′18.368″) and four in inland fields, 29.2 kilometers northeast at Lafayette Creek Wildlife Management Area in Florida (N30°32′22.999″ W86°3′26.568″; Fig. 1A). At both locations, we had recently captured live P. polionotus. Over the course of the experiment, we moved each of the eight transects four times, for a total of 32 transects, each left in place for 72 h. Within each habitat (beach or inland), simultaneous transects were set a minimum of 100 m apart or end to end, and consecutive sets of transects were set a minimum of 500 m apart. Transects consisted of 14 light and 14 dark models set out in a random order, spaced 10 m apart, and placed in the most open soil patch available. Models were checked every 24 h, and attacked models were replaced with an identical type to maintain light:dark ratios. This design resulted in a total of 2688 “model-nights.”

Predation on dark and light models on dark and light soil. (A) Location of experiments in dark inland habitat at Lafayette Creek Wildlife Area and light beach habitat at Topsail Hill State Park in Florida. (B) Typical light and dark models on soil at dark and light locations and the corresponding proportion of predation events occurring in each category.

1

Predation on dark and light models on dark and light soil. (A) Location of experiments in dark inland habitat at Lafayette Creek Wildlife Area and light beach habitat at Topsail Hill State Park in Florida. (B) Typical light and dark models on soil at dark and light locations and the corresponding proportion of predation events occurring in each category.

PREDATION SCORES

Predation events were identified as models that had been obviously attacked (based on the presence of tooth marks, bill marks or other imprints) or clearly picked up and carried in the presence of predator tracks. We were unable to find 12 models, which we identified as missing. These data were not included in selection estimates, but could be included in survival analyses for which they were considered “censored data.” Five models had imprints that we could not assign to a predator class (i.e., mammalian or avian); however, the results of the analyses did not differ when these data were omitted, so they were included (and labeled “ambiguous”). We also excluded five models that showed evidence of nonpredatory rodent gnawing.

MODEL AND SOIL BRIGHTNESS

We collected soil samples from around each deployed model and measured soil brightness. Brightness was measured similarly for models and four representative museum specimens of each subspecies (Fig. S1). Specifically, we measured reflectance across the light spectrum visible to most predators (300–700 nm; Bennett and Cuthill 1994), using a USB2000 spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL). We calculated overall brightness as the average reflectance across these wavelengths (following Mullen and Hoekstra 2008). Because the models were exposed to a natural community of predators, we did not take into account the visual sensitivity of any particular receiver, which can provide a more accurate measure of camouflage (see Stevens 2007). To account for heterogeneity within a sample, we averaged across multiple independent measurements; 10 for soil and 22 for mouse models and specimens (14 from the shoulder to the rump laterally on the left [1–7] and right [8–14], and 8 dorsally from the forehead to the base of the tail [15–22]). The average brightness for each soil and model sample was used in subsequent analyses. The measurements obtained for real mice fall within the range of those obtained for model mice (Fig. S2).

SELECTION ESTIMATES

We calculated the proportion of attacked mice that mismatched their habitat as a percentage (number of noncryptic attacked mice relative to total number of mice attacked; Table 1) and measured significance using G-tests with 1:1 expected proportions. We next estimated the strength of this selection against conspicuous mice in both habitats by calculating a selection index (SI; Dice 1947, 1949). Specifically, the selection index quantifies the relative survival of two phenotypes, which are initially in equal abundance—simply (a_−_b)/(a+b), in which “_a_” and “_b_” are the numbers of attacked individuals for each phenotypic class. The SI estimate is particularly appropriate here because we deployed equal numbers of light and dark models, the phenotype is discrete (cryptic vs. noncryptic), and we are interested in relative survival based on color differences. Finally, we used survival analysis to measure the cumulative survival probabilities of cryptic and noncryptic models overall and then separately in each habitat. Specifically, we used a Kaplan–Meier estimator (1958) to describe the survival functions, a log rank test (Mantel and Haenszel 1959; Cox 1972) to determine differences between survival functions, and Cox regression (Cox 1972; Cuthill et al. 2005) to test for a significant effect of habitat. This approach allowed us to estimate the comparative risk of a predation event in both habitats over time.

1

Location (dark or light habitat), number and type (dark or light) of models attacked, and class of predator (mammalian, avian or ambiguous).

Transect Dark model-light habitat Noncryptic Dark model-dark habitat Cryptic Light model-dark habitat Noncryptic Light model-light habitat Cryptic
L-1 2 (mamm/avian)
D-1 1 (mammalian) 1(mammalian)
D-2 1 (avian)
L-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-5 1 (mammalian)
D-7 1 (avian)
D-8 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
L-9 1 (avian) 1 (mammalian)
D-9 1 (avian)
D-10 2 (mamm/ambig)
L-11 1 (avian)
D-11 1 (ambiguous)
D-12 1 (ambiguous)
D-14 4 (3 mamm/1 avian)
L-15 1 (avian)
D-15 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
D-16 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
Total 5 5 16 2
Transect Dark model-light habitat Noncryptic Dark model-dark habitat Cryptic Light model-dark habitat Noncryptic Light model-light habitat Cryptic
L-1 2 (mamm/avian)
D-1 1 (mammalian) 1(mammalian)
D-2 1 (avian)
L-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-5 1 (mammalian)
D-7 1 (avian)
D-8 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
L-9 1 (avian) 1 (mammalian)
D-9 1 (avian)
D-10 2 (mamm/ambig)
L-11 1 (avian)
D-11 1 (ambiguous)
D-12 1 (ambiguous)
D-14 4 (3 mamm/1 avian)
L-15 1 (avian)
D-15 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
D-16 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
Total 5 5 16 2

1

Location (dark or light habitat), number and type (dark or light) of models attacked, and class of predator (mammalian, avian or ambiguous).

Transect Dark model-light habitat Noncryptic Dark model-dark habitat Cryptic Light model-dark habitat Noncryptic Light model-light habitat Cryptic
L-1 2 (mamm/avian)
D-1 1 (mammalian) 1(mammalian)
D-2 1 (avian)
L-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-5 1 (mammalian)
D-7 1 (avian)
D-8 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
L-9 1 (avian) 1 (mammalian)
D-9 1 (avian)
D-10 2 (mamm/ambig)
L-11 1 (avian)
D-11 1 (ambiguous)
D-12 1 (ambiguous)
D-14 4 (3 mamm/1 avian)
L-15 1 (avian)
D-15 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
D-16 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
Total 5 5 16 2
Transect Dark model-light habitat Noncryptic Dark model-dark habitat Cryptic Light model-dark habitat Noncryptic Light model-light habitat Cryptic
L-1 2 (mamm/avian)
D-1 1 (mammalian) 1(mammalian)
D-2 1 (avian)
L-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-4 1 (ambiguous)
D-5 1 (mammalian)
D-7 1 (avian)
D-8 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
L-9 1 (avian) 1 (mammalian)
D-9 1 (avian)
D-10 2 (mamm/ambig)
L-11 1 (avian)
D-11 1 (ambiguous)
D-12 1 (ambiguous)
D-14 4 (3 mamm/1 avian)
L-15 1 (avian)
D-15 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
D-16 1 (mammalian) 1 (mammalian)
Total 5 5 16 2

To explore the pattern of selection within a habitat, we measured the difference in brightness between each model and its local soil environment (except cases in which the models were not recovered). To specifically test the hypothesis that selection was highest against the most conspicuous models, we treated the difference in model-soil brightness as a single trait, and estimated selection using relative survival of all models as an estimate of fitness. Attacked models had a fitness of 0, nonattacked models a fitness of 1, and relative fitness was estimated by dividing these estimates by mean survival of the population. Following the approach of Lande and Arnold (1983), we calculated the selection gradient as a quadratic regression of the standardized model-soil difference values on relative “survival” and doubled the quadratic coefficient to more accurately estimate the selection coefficient (Stinchcombe et al. 2008). We determined significance using logistic regression (Fairbairn and Preziosi 1996) and a one-tailed test. To visualize the fitness surface, we binned model-soil differences into categories of five and fit a cubic spline through these values (using a λ= 1.3; Schluter 1988; Schluter and Nychka 1994).

Results

Survival was lower in conspicuous models (Fig. 2A; _P_= 0.056). Specifically, 75% of all predation events occurred on mice that did not match their substrate (Fig. 1B), representing a significantly higher rate of predation (_G_= 6.27, _P_= 0.01) and a large selective disadvantage (SI = 0.50, χ2, _P_= 0.01; SI varies from 0 to 1). In other words, conspicuous models are three times more likely to experience predation. Overall predation rate was lower in the beach habitat (Fig. 2B; _P_= 0.01), where active predator control is practiced to protect threatened/endangered beach populations. Yet, we still found a significant difference in the survival functions among all four comparisons (Fig. 2B; _P_= 0.01)—cryptic models had higher survival than noncryptic models both overall and in each habitat. Within a habitat, we found evidence that nonlinear selection favors crypsis. Because the degree of matching between the brightness of the models and their local substrate varied, we could measure predation within the light habitat. Models that were both lighter and darker than their local environment experienced a lower rate of survival than models that were better matched (stabilizing selection gradient γ=−0.034, _P_= 0.04; Fig. 3). Finally, we used tooth or bill imprints to determine the natural predators and their relative predation rates (Table 1): 54% of attacks were by mammalian carnivores (i.e., coyotes, foxes, or domestic cats) and 26% by avian predators (i.e., owls, raptors, or herons). The remaining 18% had ambiguous marks (scratches or tears to the ears or tail). Although sample sizes are small, these data suggest that natural predators may be adept at detecting even slight variations in mouse color relative to their substrate and may explain why beach mice retain some pigment on their dorsum.

Survival functions for cryptic and noncryptic models (A) across both dark inland and light beach soils and (B) in each habitat separately, in which open symbols indicate cryptic models, and solid symbols represent noncryptic models. “Day” is the number of days a model was exposed to predators. “Cumulative survival” was as calculated using the Kaplan–Meier estimator.

2

Survival functions for cryptic and noncryptic models (A) across both dark inland and light beach soils and (B) in each habitat separately, in which open symbols indicate cryptic models, and solid symbols represent noncryptic models. “Day” is the number of days a model was exposed to predators. “Cumulative survival” was as calculated using the Kaplan–Meier estimator.

The fitness surface, as calculated using a cubic spline, suggesting reduced survival when models are either much darker or much lighter than their local substrate. Dashed lines indicate bootstrap-calculated standard errors. To best visualize the data, we binned the model-soil difference (squares) in multiples of 5, thus each square may represent more than one model. Bins in which there were no attacks have a “probability of survival” of one, whereas those in which attacks occurred fall below one, proportional to the frequency of attacked models within that bin.

3

The fitness surface, as calculated using a cubic spline, suggesting reduced survival when models are either much darker or much lighter than their local substrate. Dashed lines indicate bootstrap-calculated standard errors. To best visualize the data, we binned the model-soil difference (squares) in multiples of 5, thus each square may represent more than one model. Bins in which there were no attacks have a “probability of survival” of one, whereas those in which attacks occurred fall below one, proportional to the frequency of attacked models within that bin.

Discussion

We found that cryptic individuals had a clear and strong survival advantage over conspicuous individuals, who were three times more likely to be attacked. This result was consistent across both the light, sparsely vegetated beach environment and the darker, more densely vegetated, inland habitat. Although the overall rate of predation was higher in the dark inland habitats, in relative terms, it was equally disadvantageous to be a light mouse in dark habitat as a dark mouse in light habitat. Further, mismatched individuals were selected against by both visually hunting avian predators and mammalian predators.

We also were able to document the selective advantage of substrate matching at a finer scale by examining predation in a single environment. Here, we focused on the evolution of light coloration in the beach environment because: (1) we expected selection for color matching to be stronger than in mainland habitat as there is less vegetative cover, (2) the light phenotype is derived and may have evolved recently (McNeil 1950), and (3) the genetic changes responsible for the evolution of light color have recently been identified (Hoekstra et al. 2006; Steiner et al. 2007; Mullen and Hoekstra 2008). In the light-substrate beach environment, most attacked mice were dark, but some light models also were attacked. We found that these light-colored models were all much lighter than their local substrate. In other words, selection acts against mice that are either too dark or too light relative to their background. This result not only demonstrates that there is selection on subtle color phenotypes within a habitat, but also suggests a mechanism for the substantial and rapid phenotypic divergence among beach mouse subspecies (Mullen et al. 2009).

Together our data demonstrate that cryptic coloration in Peromyscus confers a selective advantage in nature and show that color matching is driven by visually hunting predators. These results not only provide field-collected empirical evidence for a century-old assumption, but also provide the ecological underpinning for recent molecular-genetic studies that have identified the genes, and in some cases mutations, responsible for the evolution of cryptic coloration in these mice (Hoekstra et al. 2006; Steiner et al. 2007). The results of this study provide the ecological context that makes color matching in Peromyscus one of the most complete examples—from genes to ecology—of natural selection in the wild.

Associate Editor: C. Jiggins

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank A. Herrell for help with model construction, E. Jacobs-Palmer for help in the field, B. Langerhans and J. Stinchcombe for advice on data analysis, J. Losos for insightful discussion, J. Losos and two anonymous reviewers for comments on the manuscript, and the National Science Foundation (DEB-0919190) and the Harvard Museum of Comparative Zoology (MCZ) Putnam Expedition Grant Program for funding. J. Larson was supported by a MCZ Grant for Undergraduate Research.

LITERATURE CITED

Brodie

,

E. D.

1993

.

Differential avoidance of coral snake banded patterns by free-ranging avian predators in Costa Rica.

Evolution

47

:

227

235

.

Brodie

,

E. D.

, and

F. J.

Janzen

.

1995

.

Experimental studies of coral snake mimicry—generalized avoidance of ringed snake patterns by free-ranging avian predators

.

Funct. Ecol.

9

:

186

190

.

Bennett

,

A. T. D.

, and

I. C.

Cuthill

.

1994

.

Ultraviolet vision in birds: what is its function?

Vision Res.

34

:

1471

1478

.

Chlao

,

C. C.

,

C.

Chubb

, and

R. T.

Hanlon

.

2007

.

Interactive effects of size, contrast, intensity and configuration of background objects in evoking disruptive camouflage in cuttlefish

.

Vision Res.

47

:

2223

2235

.

Cox

,

D. R.

1972

.

Regression models and life tables

.

J. R. Stat. Soc. B

34

:

187

220

.

Cuthill

,

I. C.

,

M.

Stevens

,

J.

Sheppard

,

T.

Maddocks

,

C. A.

Parraga

, and

T. S.

Troscianko

.

2005

.

Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching

.

Nature

434

:

72

74

.

Darwin

,

C.

1859

.

On the origin of species by means of natural selection or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life

,

John Murray

, London.

Dice

,

L. R.

1940

.

Ecologic and genetic variability within species of Peromyscus

.

Am. Nat.

74

:

212

221

.

Dice

,

L. R.

1947

.

Effectiveness of selection by owls of deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus)

.

Contrib. Lab. Vert. Biol. Univ. Mich.

34

:

1

20

.

Dice

,

L. R.

1949

.

The selection index and its test of significance

.

Evolution

3

:

262

265

.

Fairbairn

,

D. J.

, and

R. F.

Preziosi

.

1996

.

Sexual selection and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in the waterstrider, Aquarius remigis

.

Evolution

50

:

1549

1559

.

Haldane

,

J. B. S.

1948

.

The theory of a cline

.

J. Genet.

48

:

277

284

.

Hoekstra

,

H. E.

,

R. J.

Hirschmann

,

R. J.

Bundey

,

P.

Insel

, and

J. P.

Crossland

.

2006

.

A single amino acid mutation contributes to adaptive color pattern in beach mice

.

Science

313

:

101

104

.

Husak

,

J. F.

,

J. M.

Macedonia

,

S. F.

Fox

, and

R. C.

Sauceda

.

2006

.

Predation cost of conspicuous male coloration in collared lizards (Crotaphytus collaris): an experimental test using clay-covered model lizards

.

Ethology

112

:

572

580

.

Ioannou

,

C. C.

, and

J.

Krause

.

2009

.

Interactions between background matching and motion during visual detection can explain why cryptic animals keep still

.

Biol. Lett.

5

:

191

193

.

Kaplan

,

E. L.

, and

P.

Meier

.

1958

.

Nonparametric estimation from incomplete observations

.

J. Am. Stat. Assoc.

53

:

457

481

.

Kiltie

,

R. A.

, and

A. F.

Laine

.

1992

.

Visual textures, machine vision and animal camouflage

.

Trends Ecol. Evol.

7

:

163

167

.

Kuchta

,

S. R.

2005

.

Experimental support for aposematic coloration in the salamander Ensatina eschscholtzii xanthoptica: implications for mimicry of Pacific newts

.

Copeia

2

:

265

271

.

Lande

,

R.

, and

S. J.

Arnold

.

1983

.

The measurement of selection on correlated characters

.

Evolution

37

:

1210

1226

.

Mantel

,

N.

, and

W.

Haenszel

.

1959

.

Statistical aspects of the analysis of data from retrospective studies

.

J. Natl. Cancer Inst.

22

:

719

748

.

McNeil

,

F. S.

1950

.

Pleistocene shorelines in Florida and Georgia

.

U.S. Geological Survey

221

:

59

107

.

Mullen

,

L. M.

, and

H. E.

Hoekstra

.

2008

.

Natural selection along an environmental gradient: a classic cline in mouse pigmentation

.

Evolution

62

:

1555

1570

.

Mullen

,

L. M.

,

S. N.

Vignieri

,

J. A.

Gore

, and

H. E.

Hoekstra

.

2009

.

Adaptive basis of geographic variation: genetic, phenotypic and environmental differentiation among beach mouse populations

.

Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B

276

:

3809

3818

.

Noonan

,

B. P.

, and

A. A.

Comeault

.

2009

.

The role of predator selection on polymorphic aposematic poison frogs

.

Biol. Lett.

5

:

51

54

.

Pfennig

,

D. W.

,

W. R.

Harcombe

, and

K. S.

Pfennig

.

2001

.

Frequency dependant Batesian mimicry

.

Nature

410

:

323

.

Saporito

,

R. A.

,

R.

Zuercher

,

M.

Roberts

,

K. G.

Gerow

, and

M. A.

Donnelly

.

2007

.

Experimental evidence for aposematism in the dendrobatid poison frog Oophaga pumilio

.

Copiea

4

:

1006

1011

.

Schluter

,

D.

1988

.

Estimating the form of natural selection on a quantitative trait

.

Evolution

42

:

849

861

.

Schluter

,

D.

, and

D.

Nychka

.

1994

.

Exploring fitness surfaces

.

Am. Nat.

143

:

597

616

.

Shepard

,

D. B.

2007

.

Habitat but not body shape affects predator attack frequency on lizard models in the Brazilian Cerrado

.

Herpetologica

63

:

193

202

.

Steiner

,

C. C.

,

J. N.

Weber

, and

H. E.

Hoekstra

.

2007

.

Adaptive variation in beach mice produced by two interacting pigmentation genes

.

PLoS Biol.

5

:

1880

1889

.

Stevens

,

M.

2007

.

Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration

.

Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B

274

:

1457

1464

.

Stevens

,

M.

, and

S.

Merilaita

.

2009

.

Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives

.

Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B

1516

:

423

427

.

Stinchcombe

,

J. R.

,

A. F.

Agrawal

,

P. A.

Hohenlohe

,

S. J.

Arnold

, and

M. W.

Blows

.

2008

.

Estimating nonlinear selection gradients using quadratic regression coefficients: double or nothing?

Evolution

62

:

2435

2440

.

Sumner

,

F. B.

1929a

.

The analysis of a concrete case of intergradation between two subspecies

.

Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

15

:

110

120

.

Sumner

,

F. B.

1929b

.

The analysis of a concrete case of intergradation between two subspecies. II. Additional data and interpretations

.

Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

15

:

481

493

.

Webster

,

R. J.

,

A.

Callahan

,

J. G. J.

Godin

, and

T. N.

Sherrat

.

2009

.

Behaviourally mediated crypsis in two nocturnal moths with contrasting appearance

.

Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B

1516

:

503

510

.

© 2010, Society for the Study of Evolution

Supplementary data

Citations

Views

Altmetric

Metrics

Total Views 1,616

1,287 Pageviews

329 PDF Downloads

Since 1/1/2023

Month: Total Views:
January 2023 17
February 2023 42
March 2023 18
April 2023 23
May 2023 24
June 2023 6
July 2023 21
August 2023 48
September 2023 266
October 2023 125
November 2023 49
December 2023 45
January 2024 169
February 2024 156
March 2024 114
April 2024 40
May 2024 63
June 2024 58
July 2024 49
August 2024 74
September 2024 91
October 2024 61
November 2024 57

Citations

143 Web of Science

×

Email alerts

Citing articles via

More from Oxford Academic