To Do or Not to Do: The Neural Signature of Self-Control (original) (raw)

Brief Communications

Journal of Neuroscience 22 August 2007, 27 (34) 9141-9145; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0924-07.2007

Loading

Abstract

Voluntary action is fundamental to human existence. Recent research suggests that volition involves a specific network of brain activity, centered on the fronto-median cortex. An important but neglected aspect of intentional action involves the decision whether to act or not. This decision process is crucial in daily life because it allows us to form intentions without necessarily implementing them. In the present study, we investigate the neural correlates of intentionally inhibiting actions using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Our data show that a specific area of the fronto-median cortex is more strongly activated when people prepare manual actions but then intentionally cancel them, compared with when they prepare and then complete the same actions. Our results suggest that the human brain network for intentional action includes a control structure for self-initiated inhibition or withholding of intended actions. The mental control of action has an enduring scientific interest, linked to the philosophical concept of “free will.” Our results identify a candidate brain area that reflects the crucial decision to do or not to do.

Introduction

The ability to form intentions and translate them into action is characteristically human. The human brain contains a network for intentional action, centered on the fronto-median cortex (Ball et al., 1999; Cunnington et al., 2002; Lau et al., 2004; Nachev et al., 2005). This network forms intentions about when and how to act (Libet et al., 1983; Haggard and Eimer, 1999) and also generates the conscious experience of intending (Fried et al., 1991; Lau et al., 2004). This network can partly be separated from brain areas that coordinate responses to external stimuli (Goldberg, 1985). Responding to external stimuli primarily involves a circuit including the parietal lobes and lateral premotor areas (Ammon and Gandevia, 1990). In contrast, when people make intentional actions that are endogenous rather than related to an immediate stimulus, a network of medial frontal areas is more strongly involved, including the presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA) and the cingulate motor area (CMA) (Ball et al., 1999; Cunnington et al., 2002; Lau et al., 2004; Nachev et al., 2005).

Human action generally involves selection. In the case of intentional action, the selection is internally generated, rather than specified by external stimuli. Previous studies focused on selecting when to perform a specific action (Jahanshahi et al., 1995; Jenkins et al., 2000; Nachev et al., 2005) or on how to achieve a desired action goal by selecting between alternative possible movements (Dirnberger et al., 1998; Lau et al., 2004; Waszak et al., 2005). However, few studies have considered the more fundamental process of selecting whether to make an action or not. Nevertheless, this decision whether to act often has critical consequences. For example, there is a clear distinction between intending to hit someone and actually hitting them. Many human societies acknowledge this distinction by requiring both physical action (the actus reus of Roman Law) and conscious intention (mens rea) to attribute legal and moral responsibility. Humans therefore have a widely acknowledged ability to make or withhold actions, on which society and culture rely but the neural basis of which remains poorly understood.

The frontal lobes have previously been identified with inhibition of responses to external stimuli (Konishi et al., 1998; Rubia et al., 2001; Aron et al., 2003). However, it remains unclear whether the endogenous decision to execute or inhibit an intended action involves an analogous system for endogenous inhibition of intentional action. In the present experiment, we investigated the brain processes associated with endogenous inhibition using an established method for reporting the conscious experience of intentional action in combination with event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Nineteen participants were recruited in a behavioral pre-experiment. We selected 15 participants on the basis of this pre-experiment. Selection was not based on behavioral data but on an informal interview in which we asked participants how they performed the task and followed the instructions. Four subjects reported that they were not able to follow the instructions and were excluded. The remaining 15 subjects (seven males and eight females; mean age, 26.06 years) had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of the subjects had a history of neurological, major medical, or psychiatric disorders. All were right handed, as assessed by the Edinburgh Inventory. One additional subject was excluded because of excessive head movement during scanning.

Design.

In the present experiment (Fig. 1) participants were instructed to freely decide when to execute a simple key press while observing a rotating clock hand (Libet et al., 1983). Furthermore, they were told to cancel the intended response at the last possible moment in some trials that they freely selected. After each trial, they had to determine the clock position when they formed the intention to act. Our interest focused on the comparison between trials in which subjects intentionally inhibited the action and trials in which they intentionally executed it.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Schematic drawing of the experimental conditions. In the action condition, participants had to initiate a willed action and judge the onset of their intention. In the inhibition condition, they had to initiate an action but then inhibit it in the last moment. Again, they were required to judge the onset of the intention to act. In the tone condition, a tone was presented, and participants had to judge the onset of the tone. max., Maximum.

The fMRI experiment consisted of six experimental blocks with 20 trials each. In four blocks, participants had to press a key (action condition) or inhibit the key press (inhibition condition). In the other two blocks, participants had to respond to a tone (tone condition).

Each trial began with a clock face appearing on the screen. Participants initiated the trial by pressing a key with their left index finger. After a random delay of 500–2000 ms, the clock hand appeared at a random position and began to rotate once (period, 3000 ms). The clock hand always made at least one full turn. In the action and inhibition trials, participants had two additional rotation cycles to initiate the key press (action trials) or intend the key press and afterward inhibit it (inhibition). After a key press in the action condition, the clock hand continued to rotate for another 1500–3000 ms and then disappeared. In inhibition trials, the hand simply disappeared after three rotations. Participants reported the time point when they decided to act by using a trackball to reposition a cursor at the relevant position on the clock face. They operated with their right hand. Participants had a 10 s time period to operate the trackball. In tone blocks, participants started the trial as in action blocks. However, in this condition, a tone was presented during the two rotations after the first rotation. The timing of the tone was yoked to the key-press action in action trials. Participants did not make voluntary key presses and had to indicate the onset of the tone with the trackball. Other details were as for action trials. After each trial, a random interval between 1000 and 3000 ms occurred before the next trial started.

MRI scanning procedure.

The experiment was performed on a 3T scanner (Medspec 30/100; Bruker, Ettlingen, Germany). Twenty axial slices (19.2 cm field of view, 64 × 64 matrix, 4 mm thickness, 1 mm spacing), parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure plane and covering the entire brain, were acquired using a single-shot, gradient-recalled echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence (repetition time, 2000 ms; echo time, 30 ms, 90° flip angle). Before the functional runs, 20 corresponding anatomical modified driven equilibrium Fourier transformation (MDEFT) slices and 20 EPI–T1 slices were acquired. Stimuli were displayed by a liquid crystal display projector on a backprojection screen mounted in the bore of the magnet behind the participants' head.

fMRI analysis.

Analysis of fMRI data was performed using the LIPSIA software package. Data were filtered using a spatial Gaussian filter with σ = 1. A temporal high-pass filter with a cutoff frequency of 1/80 Hz was used for baseline correction. In addition, a global scaling was performed. All functional data sets were individually registered into three-dimensional space using the participants' individual high-resolution anatomical images. The two-dimensional anatomical MDEFT slices, geometrically aligned with the functional slices, were used to compute a transformation matrix containing rotational and translational parameters that register the anatomical slices with the three-dimensional reference T1 data set. These transformation matrices were normalized to the standard Talairach brain size by linear scaling and finally applied to the individual functional data. The statistical evaluation was performed using the general linear model for serially autocorrelated observations. The design matrix for event-related analysis was created using a model of the hemodynamic response with a variable delay. The trigger for the event-related analysis was the subject's judgment of the time at which they experienced the intention to act, in the case of action and inhibition trials, or the judgment of tone onset in the case of tone trials. In inhibition trials, the temporal judgment was ambiguous because it could be related to the first or second rotation of the clock hand in which the subject was instructed to perform actions. Therefore, we put the trigger in the middle of both possible time points. The model equation was convolved with a Gaussian kernel with a dispersion of 4 s full-width at half-maximum. Contrast maps were generated for each participant. A one-sample t test of contrast maps across participants (random-effects model) was computed to ascertain whether observed differences between conditions were significantly different. Subsequently, t values were transformed into z scores. We only report cortical regions with a z < 0.001 and a volume size lager than 225 mm3 corresponding to five measured adjacent voxels (3 × 3 × 5 mm).

Results

Subjects reported the subjective experience of deciding to initiate action a mean of −141 ms before the key press on action trials (Libet et al., 1983). The mean proportion of inhibition trials was 45.5%, but there were large interindividual differences, with the proportion of inhibition trials ranging from 28 to 62%.

To determine the brain areas that were involved in inhibition of intentional action, we contrasted inhibition trials with action trials. This contrast yielded activation in the dorsal fronto-median cortex (dFMC; Brodmann's area 9) (Fig. 2A), the left and right anterior ventral insula (Fig. 2B), and the right superior temporal sulcus (STS) (Table 1).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

A, Activation in the dFMC for the contrast of inhibition versus action trials. The _z_-map is thresholded at z > 3.09 (p < 0.001). B, Activation in the left anterior ventral insula for the contrast of inhibition versus action trials.

Table 1.

Anatomical location of brain activation for inhibition versus action

The reverse contrast between action trials and inhibition trials yielded activation in a number of motor-related areas such as the primary sensorimotor cortex and the cerebellum but no activation in medial or lateral prefrontal cortex. The absence of any significant difference in areas known to be involved in preparation of voluntary action, such as pre-SMA and SMA (Cunnington et al., 2003), suggests that subjects prepared intentional actions equally both in the action trials and in the inhibition trials. This is consistent with previous reports of comparable readiness potentials in action and in inhibition trials. Our conclusion was also supported by separately comparing both action and inhibition trials with control “tone” trials in which subjects never prepared or made actions. Both contrasts yielded a significant (p < 0.001) activation focus in the SMA/pre-SMA. The peak of the preparation-related activation was in a very similar location for the action trials (x = −1, y = −5, z = 59) and for the inhibition trials (x = 4, y = 0, z = 62). Parameter estimates for the pre-SMA (x = −1, y = −5, z = 59) show no significant difference in activation strength for the action and inhibition condition (t(13) = 0.76; p = 0.45) (Fig. 3A). Moreover, this preparation-related activation was spatially quite distinct from the dFMC activation found when contrasting inhibition trials with action trials (x = −1, y = 42, z = 32).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

A, Parameter estimates for the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal in the pre-SMA for the three experimental conditions (inhibition, action, tone). B, Parameter estimates for the BOLD signal in the anterior fronto-median cortex (aFMC) for the three experimental conditions (inhibition, action, tone).

We hypothesized that the level of neural activity in any putative area for endogenous inhibition should increase the probability of inhibiting action. We therefore correlated each subject's percentage of inhibition trials with their inhibition-related activation, defined as the difference between dFMC activations for inhibition and action trials. This showed a positive correlation (r = 0.47; p < 0.05, one-tailed): participants with stronger inhibition-related dFMC activation inhibited actions more frequently.

Finally, we were interested in the relationship between inhibition-related dFMC activation and activation of primary motor cortex. If the dFMC is critical for the endogeneous inhibition of action, then its activation should be negatively related to activation of motor execution areas, such as primary motor cortex. Therefore, we expected a negative correlation for the inhibition-related dFMC activation and the activation in primary motor cortex in inhibition trials. Furthermore, this correlation should be modulated by the success of the putative inhibition process: strong inhibition should both increase the number of inhibition trials and strengthen the relationship between dFMC and primary motor activation. Activation in the dFMC and the primary motor cortex were significantly negatively correlated across subjects when we controlled for each subject's number of inhibition trials (partial r = −0.65; p < 0.01).

Discussion

Our data suggest that inhibition of intentional actions involves a fronto-median cortical area. This area is distinct from the areas that generate intentional actions, attend to intentions, or select between alternatives. A number of studies have found the SMA/pre-SMA and the CMA to be involved in intentional action (Ball et al., 1999; Cunnington et al., 2002; Lau et al., 2004; Nachev et al., 2005). However, the inhibition-related activation in the present study is too dorsal to be considered as part of the CMA and clearly too anterior to be related to the SMA/pre-SMA complex, as indicated by probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps (Eickhoff et al., 2005).

The role of the dFMC in intentional inhibition of action

Cognitive models of inhibition have focused on inhibition of prepotent responses to external stimuli (Logan et al., 1984; Cohen et al., 1990). An important distinction is made between “lateral” competitive interaction between alternative representations at a single level (Rumelhart and McClelland, 1986) and inhibitory top-down control signals from hierarchically higher brain areas (Norman and Shallice, 1986). The first idea would be consistent with a general decision process being involved. If the dFMC decides between action and inhibition by a competitive interaction process, then representations corresponding to the possibilities of action and to non-action should initially both be active, leading to activation in both action trials and inhibition trials. Our finding of minimal dFMC activation in action trials (Fig. 3B) argues against a view of endogenous inhibition based on competitive interaction between alternatives and thus is also not consistent with the idea of the dFMC being involved in a general decision process. In contrast, our result is consistent with a specific top-down control signal gating the neural pathways linking intention to action. This view is supported by the negative correlation between dFMC activation and primary motor cortex activation.

This point is also supported by the anatomical finding that activation related to inhibition was located more anteriorly than the activation related to intentional action. The prefrontal cortex shows an anterior–posterior functional gradient (Dreher et al., 2002; Forstmann et al., 2005). More anterior regions are related to higher-order intentionality, and more posterior regions are more closely related to generating the physical action itself. We therefore suggest that inhibition of intentional action involves processes that are located higher in the hierarchy of control than the development of the immediate action plan itself. Moreover, the areas involved in inhibition of intentional action have been identified by meta-analyses as playing a wider role in evaluation and appraisal of material related to the self, including but not restricted to motor acts (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004).

We also found a positive correlation across individuals between inhibition-related dFMC activation and the frequency of inhibited actions. In the psychology of individual differences, disinhibited and impulsive behavior is a crucial marker for specific personality traits that have been related to antisocial and criminal behavior (Swann et al., 2002). However, the brain mechanisms involved in such behavior are still poorly understood. Our findings suggest specific brain regions may be involved. Furthermore, inhibition of intentional actions may be closer to the unconstrained situations of everyday action than classical response inhibition paradigms generally used to investigate such behavior (Horn et al., 2003).

The anterior insula and the feeling of “let down”

Although we expected an activation in the fronto-median cortex for the inhibition condition, the activations of the ventral insula and the STS were not predicted. Anterior insula activation has been reported in response inhibition studies (Garavan et al., 1999; Wager et al., 2005), but usually more dorsally than here. Furthermore, an activation very close to the present activation located in the mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex has been related to negative feedback in a Wisconsin Card Sorting task (Monchi et al., 2001). Meta-analytic data suggest that the ventral insula is involved in emotional rather than cognitive processes (Wager and Feldman Barrett, 2004).

This area may evaluate afferent homeostatic information from the body, leading to “subjective feeling states arising from representations of bodily responses” (Damasio, 1999; Critchley et al., 2004). Furthermore, the anterior insula has been found to be related to the feeling of frustration (Abler et al., 2005). Canceling an ongoing motor intention may result in an unresolved tension or feeling of let down. A recent study using the stop-signal paradigm is consistent with this interpretation. Ramautar et al. (2006) found stronger insula activation on unsuccessful stop-signal trials for blocks with rare stop signals compared with more frequent stop signals. They attributed this difference to the stronger feeling of failure in the rare condition. This description captures nicely the report of our debriefing in which most participants reported inhibition trials to be extremely frustrating. We suggest that insula activation represents the affective-somatic consequences of failing to implement a strong intention.

Intentional inhibition as a crucial component of intentional action

The inhibition of intentional action has been primarily neglected both in the neuroscientific literature on response inhibition and in that on intentional action. Our findings suggest that inhibition of intentional action involves cortical areas different from, and upstream from, the intentional generation and execution of action. In addition, this process of “last-minute” inhibition is compatible with a conscious experience of intending to act. Some parts of the inhibition process may occur after the intention to perform an action has become conscious. For example, Libet (Libet et al., 1983; Libet, 2004) hypothesized a “veto” allowing the conscious mind to intervene to withhold unconsciously initiated action plans. Because he could not find any identifiable neural correlate of the veto process, he suggested it could involve mind–brain causation: a “free won't” analogous to “free will.” Our data identify a clear neural basis for inhibiting intentions and thus identify the neural correlate of the veto process. The hypothesis of a special, non-neural veto process could therefore become unnecessary. Because the initiation of action is unconscious (Libet et al., 1983), then the initiation of inhibition may also be unconscious. In addition, the poor temporal resolution of fMRI means we cannot yet judge whether the inhibitory process could operate in the seemingly brief interval between the time of conscious intention and the point of no return for motor output (Libet et al., 1983; Walton et al., 2004). Our results provide the first clear neuroscientific basis for the widely held view that people can refrain from doing something even if they genuinely wish to do it (Baumeister and Exline, 1999). We speculate that the dFMC may be involved in those aspects of behavior and personality that reflect “self-control” (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004). Identifying specific brain processes and regions that are central to individuals' personalities, and are also of social and legal importance, may have profound neuroethical implications.

Footnotes

References