Motivational control of goal-directed action (original) (raw)
References
Adams, C. D., &Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,33B, 109–122. Google Scholar
Asratyan, E. A. (1974). Conditioned reflex theory and motivational behavior.Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis,34, 15–31. PubMed Google Scholar
Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., van den Berg, H., &Crombez, G. (1990). Flavor-flavor and color-flavor conditioning in humans.Learning & Motivation,21, 434–455. Article Google Scholar
Balleine, B. (1992). Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends upon incentive learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,18, 236–250. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1991). Instrumental performance following reinforcer devaluation depends upon incentive learning.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,43B, 279–296. Google Scholar
Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1992). Signalling and incentive processes in instrumental reinforcer devaluation.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,45B, 285–301. Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1991). Modulation of taste affect by hunger, caloric satiety, and sensory-specific satiety in the rat.Appetite,16, 103–120. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., Flynn, F. W., Schulkin, J., &Grill, H. J. (1984). Sodium depletion enhances salt palatability in rats.Behavioral Neuroscience98, 652–660. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C., Grill, H. J., &Norgren, R. (1981). Relation of consummatory responses and preabsorptive insulin release to palatability and learned taste aversions.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,95, 363–382. Article Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1972). A unified account of classical conditioning and operant training. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.),Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 453–482). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual motivational alternative to response-reinforcement.Behavior & Brain Sciences,1, 41–52. Article Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning.Psychological Review,95, 394–409. Article Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1975).Theory of motivation. New York: Harper & Row. Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C., Holtz, R., Dunn, T., &Hill, W. (1980). Comparison of stimulus learning and response learning in a punishment situation.Learning & Motivation,11, 78–96. Article Google Scholar
Campbell, D. H., Capaldi, E. D., Sheffer, J. D., &Bradford, J. P. (1988). An examination of the relationship between expectancy learning and preference conditioning.Learning & Motivation,19, 162–182. Article Google Scholar
Capaldi, E. D., Davidson, T. L., &Myers, D. E. (1981). Resistance to satiation: Reinforcing effects of food and eating under satiation.Learning & Motivation,12, 171–195. Article Google Scholar
Capaldi, E. D., &Myers, D. E. (1982). Taste preferences as a function of food deprivation during original taste exposure.Animal Learning & Behavior,10, 211–219. Google Scholar
Capaldi, E. D., Myers, D. E., Campbell, D. H., &Sheffer, J. D. (1983). Conditioned flavor preferences based on hunger level during original flavor exposure.Animal Learning & Behavior,11, 107–115. Google Scholar
Capaldi, E. D., Sheffer, J., &Owens, J. (1991). Food deprivation and conditioned flavor preferences based on sweetened and unsweetened foods.Animal Learning & Behavior,19, 361–368. Article Google Scholar
Capaldi, E. D., Viveiros, D. M., &Davidson, T. L. (1981). Deprivation stimulus intensity and incentive factors in the control of instrumental performance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,7, 140–149. Article Google Scholar
Colwill, R. C., &Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,11, 120–132. Article Google Scholar
Colwill, R. C., &Rescorla, R. A., (1986). Associative structures in instrumental learning. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 20, pp. 55–104). New York: Academic Press. Google Scholar
Crocetti, C. P. (1962). Drive level and response strength in the barpressing apparatus.Psychological Reports,10, 563–575. Article Google Scholar
Davidson, T. L. (1987). Learning about deprivational intensity stimuli.Behavioral Neuroscience,101, 198–208. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Davidson, T. L., Flynn, F. W., &Jarrard, L. E. (1992). Potency of food deprivation intensity cues as discriminative stimuli.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,18, 174–181. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1980).Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1986). Re-examination of the role of the instrumental contingency in the sodium-appetite irrelevant incentive effect.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,38B, 161–172. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1989). Expectancy theory in animal conditioning. In S. B. Klein & R. R. Mowrer (Eds.),Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory (pp. 279–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., &Balleine, B. (1990). Motivational control of instrumental performance following a shift from hunger to thirst.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,42B, 413–431. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1987a). Pavlovian processes in the motivational control of instrumental performance.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,38B, 201–213. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1987b). The role of the instrumental contingency in the motivational control of performance.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,39B, 77–93. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1988). Motivational control of instrumental performance: The role of prior experience of the reinforcer.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,40B, 113–134. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1989). Incentive learning and the motivational control of instrumental performance.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,41B, 99–112. Google Scholar
Dickinson, A., &Nicholas, D. J. (1983). Irrelevant incentive learning during instrumental conditioning: The role of drive-reinforcer and response-reinforcer relationships.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,35B, 249–263. Google Scholar
Eisman, E., Theios, J., &Linton, M. (1961). Habit strength as a function of drive in a bar pressing situation.Psychological Reports,9, 583–590. Article Google Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1943). Discriminative conditioning: 1. A discriminative property of conditioned anticipation.Journal of Experimental Psychology,32, 150–155. Article Google Scholar
Everitt, B. J., &Stacey, P. (1987). Studies of instrumental behavior with sexual reinforcement in male rats (Rattus norvegicus): 2. Effects of preoptic area lesions, castration, and testosterone.Journal of Comparative Psychology,101, 407–419. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Fitzsimons, J. T. (1961). Drinking by nephrectomized rats injected with various substances.Journal of Physiology,155, 563–579. PubMed Google Scholar
Garcia, J. (1989). Food for Tolman: Cognition and cathexis in concert. In T. Archer & L.-G. Nilsson (Eds.),Aversion, avoidance and anxiety (pp. 45–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
Gormezano, I., &Tait, R. W. (1976). The Pavlovian analysis of instrumental conditioning.Pavlovian Journal of Biological Sciences,11, 37–55. Google Scholar
Grice, G. R., &Davis, J. D. (1957). Effect of irrelevant thirst motivation on a response learned with food reward.Journal of Experimental Psychology,53, 347–352. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Grill, H. J., &Berridge, K. C. (1985). Taste reactivity as a measure of the neural control of palatability. In J. M. Sprague & A. N. Epstein (Eds.),Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 1–61). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Google Scholar
Grindley, G. C. (1932). The formation of a simple habit in guinea pigs.British Journal of Psychology,23, 127–147. Google Scholar
Heathers, G. L., &Arakelian, P. (1941). The relationship between strength of drive and rate of extinction of a bar-pressing reaction in the rat.Journal of General Psychology,24, 243–258. Google Scholar
Hendersen, R. W., &Graham, J. (1979). Avoidance of heat by rats: Effects of thermal context on the rapidity of extinction.Learning & Motivation,10, 351–363. Article Google Scholar
Hershberger, W. A. (1986). An approach through the looking-glass.Animal Learning & Behavior,14, 443–451. Google Scholar
Horenstein, B. R. (1951). Performance of conditioned responses as a function of strength of hunger drive.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,44, 210–224. Article Google Scholar
Hovancik, J. R. (1978). The effect of deprivation level during noncontingent pairings and instrumental learning on subsequent instrumental performance.Learning & Motivation,9, 1–15. Article Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943).Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Google Scholar
Irwin, F. W. (1971).Intentional behavior and motivation: A cognitive theory. Philadelphia: Lippincott. Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M., &Harrison, R. H. (1960). The effects of discrimination training on auditory generalization.Journal of Experimental Psychology,59, 246–253. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
King, D. L. (1979).Conditioning: An image approach. New York: Gardner. Google Scholar
Koch, S., &Daniel, W. J. (1945). The effect of satiation on behavior mediated by a habit of maximum strength.Journal of Experimental Psychology,35, 167–187. Article Google Scholar
Konorski, J. (1967).Integrative activity of the brain: An interdisciplinary approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Google Scholar
Krieckhaus, E. E., &Wolf, G. (1968). Acquisition of sodium by rats: Interaction of innate mechanisms and latent learning.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,65, 197–201. Article Google Scholar
Lopez, M., Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1992a). Incentive learning and the motivational control of instrumental performance by thirst.Animal Learning & Behavior,20, 322–328. Article Google Scholar
Lopez, M., Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1992b). Incentive learning following reinforcer devaluation is not conditional upon motivational state during re-exposure.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,45B, 265–284. Google Scholar
Meachum, C. L. (1990). The role of response-contingent incentives in lithium chloride-mediated suppression of an operant response.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,42B, 175–195. Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1963). Some reflections on the law of effect produce a new alternative to drive reduction. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 65–112). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Google Scholar
Miller, S., &Konorski, J. (1969). On a particular form of conditioned reflex.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,12, 187–189. [Translation by B. F. Skinner of: Sur une forme particulière des reflex conditionnels.Les Compte Rendus des Seances de la Société Polonaise de Biologie, 1928,49, 1155–1157]. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Morgan, M. J. (1974). Resistance to satiation.Animal Behaviour,22, 449–466. Article Google Scholar
Morrison, S. D., Mackay, C., Hurlbrink, E., Wier, J. K., Nick, M. S., &Miller, F. K. (1967). The water exchange and polyuria of rats deprived of food.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology,52, 51–67. Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1960).Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley. Book Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927).Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1932). The reply of a physiologist to a psychologist.Psychological Review,39, 91–127. Article Google Scholar
Pelchat, M. L., Grill, H. J., Rozin, P., &Jacobs, J. (1983). Quality of acquired responses to taste by_Rattus norvegicus_ depends upon type of associated discomfort.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,97, 140–153. Google Scholar
Perin, C. T. (1942). Behavior potentiality as a joint function of the amount of training and the degree of hunger at the time of extinction.Journal of Experimental Psychology,30, 93–113. Article Google Scholar
Peterson, D., &McHose, J. H. (1980). The residual deprivation effect in instrumental conditioning as a function of deprivation level at the time of exposure to the reinforcer.Animal Learning & Behavior,8, 642–646. Article Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1992). Depression of an instrumental response by a single devaluation of its outcome.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,44B, 123–136. Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., &Solomon, R. L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationship between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning.Psychological Review,74, 151–182. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Revusky, S. H. (1967). Hunger level during food consumption: Effects on subsequent preference.Psychonomic Science,7, 109–110. Google Scholar
Revusky, S. H. (1968). Effects of thirst level during consumption of flavored water on subsequent preference.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,66, 777–779. Article Google Scholar
Rolls, B. J., &McFarland, D. J. (1973). Hydration releases inhibition of feeding produced by intracranial angiotensin.Physiology & Behavior,11, 881–884. Article Google Scholar
Rozin, P., &Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust.Psychological Review,94, 23–41. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Sackett, R. S. (1939). The effect of strength of drive at the time of extinction upon resistance to extinction in rats.Journal of Comparative Psychology,27, 411–431. Article Google Scholar
Saltzman, I., &Koch, S. (1948). The effect of low intensities of hunger on the behavior mediated by a habit of maximum strength.Journal of Experimental Psychology,38, 347–370. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Sheffield, F. D. (1965). Relation between classical conditioning and instrumental learning. In W. F. Prokasy (Ed.),Classical conditioning: A symposium (pp. 302–322). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1932). On the rate of formation of a conditioned reflex.Journal of General Psychology,7, 274–285. Article Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1936). Conditioning and extinction and their relation to drive.Journal of General Psychology,14, 296–317. Article Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Book Google Scholar
Sutton, R. S., &Barto, A. G. (1981). An adaptive network that constructs and uses an internal model of its world.Cognition & Brain Theory,4, 217–246. Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1911).Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan. Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1978). A physiological control theory of hunger-thirst interaction. In D. A. Booth (Ed.),Hunger model (pp. 347–374). London: Academic Press. Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1949a). The nature and function of wants.Psychological Review,56, 357–369. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1949b). There is more than one kind of learning.Psychological Review,56, 144–155. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1959). Principles of purposive behavior. In S. Koch (Ed.),Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2, pp. 92–157). New York: McGraw-Hill. Google Scholar
Trapold, M. A., &Overmier, J. B. (1972). The second learning process in instrumental learning. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds),Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 427–452). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Google Scholar
Van Hemel, P. E., &Myers, J. S. (1970). Control of food-motivated instrumental behavior in water-deprived rats by prior water and saline drinking.Learning & Motivation,1, 86–94. Article Google Scholar
Verplanck, W. S., &Hayes, J. R. (1953). Eating and drinking as a function of maintenance schedule.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,46, 327–333, Article Google Scholar
Webb, W. B. (1955). Drive stimuli as cues.Psychological Reports,1, 287–298. Article Google Scholar
Wetzel, R. J. (1959). The effect of experience with a taste reward.Journal of Comparative A. Physiological Psychology,52, 267–271. Article Google Scholar
Woods, S. C., Vasselli, J. R., &Milan, K. M. (1977). Iron appetite and latent learning in rats.Physiology & Behavior,19, 623–626. Article Google Scholar
Young, P. T. (1961).Motivation and emotion. New York: Wiley. Google Scholar