John Whalen - Academia.edu (original) (raw)

Papers by John Whalen

Research paper thumbnail of Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation

Psychological Science, 1999

In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly ... more In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly attempted to produce target numbers of key presses at rates that made vocal or subvocal counting difficult or impossible. In a second task, they estimated the number of flashes in a rapid, randomly timed sequence. Congruent with the animal data, mean estimates in both tasks were proportional to target values, as was the variability in the estimates. Converging evidence makes it unlikely that subjects used verbal counting or time durations to perform these tasks. The results support the hypothesis that adult humans share with non-verbal animals a system for representing number by magnitudes that have scalar variability (a constant coefficient of variation). The mapping of numerical symbols to mental magnitudes provides a formal model of the underlying non-verbal meaning of the symbols (a model of numerical semantics).

Research paper thumbnail of Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2001

Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting conditi... more Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting condition, subjects pressed a key a numeral-specified number of times, while saying “the” at every press. The mean number of presses increased as a power function of the target number, with a constant coefficient of variation (c.v.), both within and beyond the proposed subitizing range (1–4 or 5), suggesting small numbers are represented on the same continuum as larger numbers and subject to the same noise process (scalar variability). By contrast, when subjects counted their presses out loud as fast as they could, the c.v. decreased as the inverse square root of the target value (binomial variability instead of scalar variability). The unexpected power-law relation between target value and mean number of presses in nonverbal counting suggests a new hypothesis about the development of the function relating number symbols to mental magnitudes.

Research paper thumbnail of Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation

Psychological Science, 1999

In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly ... more In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly attempted to produce target numbers of key presses at rates that made vocal or subvocal counting difficult or impossible. In a second task, they estimated the number of flashes in a rapid, randomly timed sequence. Congruent with the animal data, mean estimates in both tasks were proportional to target values, as was the variability in the estimates. Converging evidence makes it unlikely that subjects used verbal counting or time durations to perform these tasks. The results support the hypothesis that adult humans share with non-verbal animals a system for representing number by magnitudes that have scalar variability (a constant coefficient of variation). The mapping of numerical symbols to mental magnitudes provides a formal model of the underlying non-verbal meaning of the symbols (a model of numerical semantics).

Research paper thumbnail of Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2001

Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting conditi... more Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting condition, subjects pressed a key a numeral-specified number of times, while saying “the” at every press. The mean number of presses increased as a power function of the target number, with a constant coefficient of variation (c.v.), both within and beyond the proposed subitizing range (1–4 or 5), suggesting small numbers are represented on the same continuum as larger numbers and subject to the same noise process (scalar variability). By contrast, when subjects counted their presses out loud as fast as they could, the c.v. decreased as the inverse square root of the target value (binomial variability instead of scalar variability). The unexpected power-law relation between target value and mean number of presses in nonverbal counting suggests a new hypothesis about the development of the function relating number symbols to mental magnitudes.

Research paper thumbnail of The Influence of Semantic Magnitude Representations on Arithmetic: Theory, Data, and Simulation

... This problem size effect has been reported in studies of both addition (Ashcraft & Ba... more ... This problem size effect has been reported in studies of both addition (Ashcraft & Battaglia, 1978), and multiplication (Campbell, 1985; Harley, 1990; Miller, Perlmutter, & Keating, 1984), and has been reported across cultures (Geary, 1996). ...

Research paper thumbnail of Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation

Psychological Science, 1999

In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly ... more In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly attempted to produce target numbers of key presses at rates that made vocal or subvocal counting difficult or impossible. In a second task, they estimated the number of flashes in a rapid, randomly timed sequence. Congruent with the animal data, mean estimates in both tasks were proportional to target values, as was the variability in the estimates. Converging evidence makes it unlikely that subjects used verbal counting or time durations to perform these tasks. The results support the hypothesis that adult humans share with non-verbal animals a system for representing number by magnitudes that have scalar variability (a constant coefficient of variation). The mapping of numerical symbols to mental magnitudes provides a formal model of the underlying non-verbal meaning of the symbols (a model of numerical semantics).

Research paper thumbnail of Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2001

Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting conditi... more Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting condition, subjects pressed a key a numeral-specified number of times, while saying “the” at every press. The mean number of presses increased as a power function of the target number, with a constant coefficient of variation (c.v.), both within and beyond the proposed subitizing range (1–4 or 5), suggesting small numbers are represented on the same continuum as larger numbers and subject to the same noise process (scalar variability). By contrast, when subjects counted their presses out loud as fast as they could, the c.v. decreased as the inverse square root of the target value (binomial variability instead of scalar variability). The unexpected power-law relation between target value and mean number of presses in nonverbal counting suggests a new hypothesis about the development of the function relating number symbols to mental magnitudes.

Research paper thumbnail of Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation

Psychological Science, 1999

In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly ... more In non-verbal counting tasks derived from the animal literature, adult human subjects repeatedly attempted to produce target numbers of key presses at rates that made vocal or subvocal counting difficult or impossible. In a second task, they estimated the number of flashes in a rapid, randomly timed sequence. Congruent with the animal data, mean estimates in both tasks were proportional to target values, as was the variability in the estimates. Converging evidence makes it unlikely that subjects used verbal counting or time durations to perform these tasks. The results support the hypothesis that adult humans share with non-verbal animals a system for representing number by magnitudes that have scalar variability (a constant coefficient of variation). The mapping of numerical symbols to mental magnitudes provides a formal model of the underlying non-verbal meaning of the symbols (a model of numerical semantics).

Research paper thumbnail of Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2001

Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting conditi... more Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting condition, subjects pressed a key a numeral-specified number of times, while saying “the” at every press. The mean number of presses increased as a power function of the target number, with a constant coefficient of variation (c.v.), both within and beyond the proposed subitizing range (1–4 or 5), suggesting small numbers are represented on the same continuum as larger numbers and subject to the same noise process (scalar variability). By contrast, when subjects counted their presses out loud as fast as they could, the c.v. decreased as the inverse square root of the target value (binomial variability instead of scalar variability). The unexpected power-law relation between target value and mean number of presses in nonverbal counting suggests a new hypothesis about the development of the function relating number symbols to mental magnitudes.

Research paper thumbnail of The Influence of Semantic Magnitude Representations on Arithmetic: Theory, Data, and Simulation

... This problem size effect has been reported in studies of both addition (Ashcraft & Ba... more ... This problem size effect has been reported in studies of both addition (Ashcraft & Battaglia, 1978), and multiplication (Campbell, 1985; Harley, 1990; Miller, Perlmutter, & Keating, 1984), and has been reported across cultures (Geary, 1996). ...