Zoe Morris Trainor - Academia.edu (original) (raw)
Uploads
Papers by Zoe Morris Trainor
The present study tested whether the relationship between trauma and religious change, and the su... more The present study tested whether the relationship between trauma and religious change, and the subsequent effect of this change on wellbeing, depends on whether the trauma involves death. Using a large and population-representative longitudinal sample, participants’ religiosity at age 26 and 32 was measured, as was their exposure to a range of traumas (some involving death and some not) within the intervening years. Participants’ satisfaction with life was also measured at age 38. Experiencing a death related trauma (such as the death of a loved one), but not an equally traumatic event that did not involve death (such as divorce), increased private religious behaviours among those already practicing them, and increased the perceived value of religious ceremonies among those who previously saw little value in them. On the other hand, living through a death unrelated event led to less public religious involvement among those who were previously so inclined. Changes in religiosity following trauma did not predict wellbeing six years later, however. These results help to clarify the contradictory finding in the literature on trauma, religion, and wellbeing: not only does the type of trauma influence the nature of religious change, but it does so in different ways for religious and unreligious people, and for different dimensions of religiosity
Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 2019
Objective: Previous research has shown contradictory evidence for the relationship between religi... more Objective: Previous research has shown contradictory evidence for the relationship between religiosity and trauma; exposure to traumatic life events has been associated with both increases and decreases in religiosity over time. On the basis of a long theoretical tradition of linking death and religious belief and recent empirical evidence that thoughts of death may increase religiosity, we tested whether one determinant of trauma's influence on religion is the degree to which it makes death salient. Method: Using longitudinal data from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, a unique population-representative birth cohort, we tested whether the relationship between trauma and religiosity depends on whether the trauma involves death. Participants reported their private, ceremonial, and public religious behaviors at ages 26 and 32 and, at age 32, whether they had experienced any of 23 traumatic life events since age 26. Results: Experiencing the death of a loved one (but not an equally traumatic event not involving death) predicted a future increase in private religious behavior (e.g., prayer) among those already practicing such behaviors, and an increase in the importance of religious ceremonies among those with relatively little prior interest in them. On the other hand, experiencing a death-unrelated trauma predicted a future reduction in public displays of religiosity among those previously so inclined. Conclusion: The study represents a significant step in understanding religious responses to trauma, and emphasizes the importance of considering not only the nature of a trauma, but also the dimensions and practices of a victim's religiosity prior to it. Clinical Impact Statement Why, when personal tragedy strikes, do some people seek solace in religion, but others do not? Our longitudinal study shows that part of the answer involves whether a traumatic event involves death. People who experienced the death of a loved one reported more frequent prayer, and attributed greater importance to religious ceremonies, but people who experienced an equally (death-unrelated) traumatic event reduced their church attendance. The results suggest that clinicians should take account of the death-relatedness of a trauma, as well the dimensions and practices of a victim's religiosity prior to it, when predicting the value of religious coping strategies.
The present study tested whether the relationship between trauma and religious change, and the su... more The present study tested whether the relationship between trauma and religious change, and the subsequent effect of this change on wellbeing, depends on whether the trauma involves death. Using a large and population-representative longitudinal sample, participants’ religiosity at age 26 and 32 was measured, as was their exposure to a range of traumas (some involving death and some not) within the intervening years. Participants’ satisfaction with life was also measured at age 38. Experiencing a death related trauma (such as the death of a loved one), but not an equally traumatic event that did not involve death (such as divorce), increased private religious behaviours among those already practicing them, and increased the perceived value of religious ceremonies among those who previously saw little value in them. On the other hand, living through a death unrelated event led to less public religious involvement among those who were previously so inclined. Changes in religiosity following trauma did not predict wellbeing six years later, however. These results help to clarify the contradictory finding in the literature on trauma, religion, and wellbeing: not only does the type of trauma influence the nature of religious change, but it does so in different ways for religious and unreligious people, and for different dimensions of religiosity
Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 2019
Objective: Previous research has shown contradictory evidence for the relationship between religi... more Objective: Previous research has shown contradictory evidence for the relationship between religiosity and trauma; exposure to traumatic life events has been associated with both increases and decreases in religiosity over time. On the basis of a long theoretical tradition of linking death and religious belief and recent empirical evidence that thoughts of death may increase religiosity, we tested whether one determinant of trauma's influence on religion is the degree to which it makes death salient. Method: Using longitudinal data from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, a unique population-representative birth cohort, we tested whether the relationship between trauma and religiosity depends on whether the trauma involves death. Participants reported their private, ceremonial, and public religious behaviors at ages 26 and 32 and, at age 32, whether they had experienced any of 23 traumatic life events since age 26. Results: Experiencing the death of a loved one (but not an equally traumatic event not involving death) predicted a future increase in private religious behavior (e.g., prayer) among those already practicing such behaviors, and an increase in the importance of religious ceremonies among those with relatively little prior interest in them. On the other hand, experiencing a death-unrelated trauma predicted a future reduction in public displays of religiosity among those previously so inclined. Conclusion: The study represents a significant step in understanding religious responses to trauma, and emphasizes the importance of considering not only the nature of a trauma, but also the dimensions and practices of a victim's religiosity prior to it. Clinical Impact Statement Why, when personal tragedy strikes, do some people seek solace in religion, but others do not? Our longitudinal study shows that part of the answer involves whether a traumatic event involves death. People who experienced the death of a loved one reported more frequent prayer, and attributed greater importance to religious ceremonies, but people who experienced an equally (death-unrelated) traumatic event reduced their church attendance. The results suggest that clinicians should take account of the death-relatedness of a trauma, as well the dimensions and practices of a victim's religiosity prior to it, when predicting the value of religious coping strategies.