Spatial relationships and matrilineal kinship in African savanna elephant (Loxodonta africana) clans (original) (raw)
Abstract
African savanna elephants, Loxodonta africana, live in stable family groups consisting of adult females and their dependent offspring. During the dry season, “clans” consisting of several family groups typically share a common home range. We compared spatial relationships and mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) haplotypes among 14 adult female elephants within 3 clans during the dry season in northern Zimbabwe. Spatial relationships were studied by radio-tracking. Home-range similarity was quantified by correlating the estimated utilization distributions of all pairs of elephants. Clans were identified by cluster analysis of the home-range similarity values. All three clans contained at least two of the five mtDNA haplotypes that were found, indicating that clan members are not necessarily matrilineally related. Within clans, home ranges of elephants with the same haplotype were not significantly more similar to each other than those of elephants with different haplotypes. Most elephants within each clan used their shared home ranges independently of each other: the distribution of distances between their positions at any given time did not differ from the distribution expected by chance. However, 8 out of the 26 within-clan pairs exhibited long-term coordination of space use by remaining within known hearing distance of each other’s low-frequency calls significantly more often than expected by chance. At least four of these coordinated pairs consisted of animals in different family groups. Elephants in three of the four different-family pairs whose movements were coordinated had different haplotypes. Further research is needed to determine the relationship between these coordinated movements and conventionally defined bond-group behavior.
Access this article
Subscribe and save
- Get 10 units per month
- Download Article/Chapter or eBook
- 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
- Cancel anytime Subscribe now
Buy Now
Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.
Instant access to the full article PDF.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
- De Solla SR, Bonduriansky R, Brooks RJ (1999) Eliminating autocorrelation reduces biological relevance of home range estimates. J Anim Ecol 68:221–234
Article Google Scholar - Dietz EJ (1983) Permutation tests for association between two distance matrices. Syst Zool 32:21–26
Google Scholar - Doncaster CP (1990) Non-parametric estimates of interaction from radio-tracking data. J Theor Biol 143:431–443
Google Scholar - Douglas-Hamilton I (1972) On the ecology and behaviour of the African elephant. PhD Thesis, University of Oxford
- Fernando P, Lande R (2000) Molecular genetic and behavioral analysis of social organization in the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:84–91
Article Google Scholar - Fernando P, Pfrender ME, Encalada SE, Lande R (2000) Mitochondrial DNA variation, phylogeography and population structure of the Asian elephant. Heredity 84:362–372
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Guy P (1974) Feeding behaviour of the African elephant in the Sengwa Research Area of Rhodesia. MSc Thesis, University of Rhodesia, Salisbury
- Langbauer WR, Payne KB, Charif RA, Rapaport L, Osborn F (1991) African elephants respond to distant playbacks of low-frequency conspecific calls. J Exp Biol 157:35–46
Google Scholar - Larom D, Garstang M, Payne K, Raspet R, Lindeque M (1997) The influence of surface atmospheric conditions on the range and area reached by animal vocalizations. J Exp Biol 200:421–431
PubMed Google Scholar - Martin RB (1978) Aspects of elephant social organisation. Rhod Sci News 12:184–187
Google Scholar - MathWorks (1992) MATLAB Reference Guide. MathWorks, Natick, Mass
- McComb K, Moss C, Sayaliel S, Baker L (2000) Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants. Anim Behav 59:1103–1109
Article PubMed Google Scholar - McComb K, Reby D, Baker L, Moss C, Sayaliel S (2003) Long-distance communication of acoustic cues to social identity in African elephants. Anim Behav 65:317–329
Article Google Scholar - Moss CJ (1988) Elephant memories. Morrow, New York
- Moss CJ, Poole JH (1983) Relationships and social structure of African elephants. In: Hinde RA (ed) Primate social relationships: an integrated approach. Blackwell, Oxford
- Mullenbach R, Lagoda PJL, Welter C (1989) An efficient salt-chloroform extraction of DNA from blood and tissues. Trend Genet 5:391
CAS Google Scholar - Nyakaana S, Arctander P (1999) Population genetic structure of the African elephant in Uganda based on variation at mitochondrial and nuclear loci. Evidence for male-biased gene flow. Mol Ecol 8:1105–1115
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Nyakaana S, Abe EL, Arctander P, Siegismund HR (2001) DNA evidence for elephant social behaviour breakdown in Queen Elizabeth National Park, Uganda. Anim Conserv 4:231–237
Article Google Scholar - Osborn FV (1998) The ecology of crop-raiding elephants in Zimbabwe. PhD Thesis, University of Cambridge
- Payne K (1998) Silent thunder. Simon and Schuster, New York
- Poole JH (1987) Rutting behaviour in African elephants: the phenomenon of musth. Behaviour 102:283–316
Google Scholar - Poole JH, Moss CJ (1981) Musth in the African elephant, Loxodonta africana. Nature 292:830–831
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Poole JH, Payne KB, Langbauer WR Jr, Moss CJ (1988) The social contexts of some very low-frequency calls of African elephants. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22:385–392
Article Google Scholar - Roca AL, Georgiadis N, Pecon-Slattery J, O’Brien SJ (2001) Genetic evidence for two species of elephant in Africa. Science 293:1473–1477
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Schnell GD, Watt DJ, Douglas ME (1985) Statistical comparison of proximity matrices: applications in animal behaviour. Anim Behav 33:239–253
Google Scholar - Seaman DE, Powell RA (1996) An evaluation of the accuracy of kernel density estimators for home range analysis. Ecology 77:2075–2085
Google Scholar - StatSoft (1994) STATISTICA for the Macintosh. StatSoft, Tulsa, Okla
- Sukumar R (2003) The living elephants. Oxford University Press, Oxford
- Swihart RK, Slade NA (1985) Testing for independence of observations in animal movements. Ecology 66:1176–1184
Google Scholar - Thouless CR (1996) Home ranges and social organization of female elephants in northern Kenya. Afr J Ecol 34:284–297
Google Scholar - White GC, Garrott RA (1990) Analysis of wildlife radio-tracking data. Academic, New York
- Worton BJ (1989) Kernel methods for estimating the utilization distribution in home-range studies. Ecology 70:164–168
Google Scholar - Worton BJ (1995) Using Monte Carlo simulation to evaluate kernel-based home range estimators. J Wildl Manage 59:794–800
Google Scholar
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (grant no. BNS-8910482), World Wildlife Fund-US, the National Geographic Society (grant no. 3610-87), a Guggenheim Fellowship to Katharine Payne, and private contributions from the late John S. McIlhenny and Caroline Getty. The Zimbabwe Department of Parks and Wild Life Management made all of the facilities and personnel of the Sengwa Wildlife Research Institute (SWRI) available in support of the field work. This work would have been impossible without the able assistance in the field of the late Andrew Masarirevhu and twelve other game scouts of the SWRI. The late Ian Coulson, then director of the SWRI, and the entire staff of the SWRI supported this work logistically and scientifically. Loki Osborn and Lillie Wilson assisted with all aspects of the field work. Mike Kock supervised the immobilization of the elephants. Deborah Gibson flew aerial surveys. Helicopter time and fuel during collaring operations were donated by Mobil Oil Corporation. We thank Charles Walcott and Christopher Clark for their support of this work. Naomi Altman provided statistical advice. Kathy Dunsmore, Kurt Fristrup, Leila Hatch, Irby Lovette, Patricia Parker, Joyce Poole, Sandra Vehrencamp, and an anonymous reviewer made helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. The research described here complies with the current laws of Zimbabwe.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
- Bioacoustics Research Program, Laboratory of Ornithology, Cornell University, 159 Sapsucker Woods Rd., Ithaca, NY 14850, USA
Russell A. Charif & Katharine B. Payne - Dept. of Zoology, Denver Museum of Nature & Science, USA
Rob Roy Ramey II & Laura M. Brown - Pittsburgh Zoo and PPG Aquarium, USA
William R. Langbauer Jr. - Greendale, P.O. Box GD890, Harare, Zimbabwe
Rowan B. Martin
Authors
- Russell A. Charif
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Rob Roy Ramey II
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - William R. Langbauer Jr.
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Katharine B. Payne
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Rowan B. Martin
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Laura M. Brown
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding author
Correspondence toRussell A. Charif.
Additional information
Communicated by C. Nunn
Electronic Supplementary Material
Appendix
Appendix
Estimation of mean location error
We estimated the mean location error for each tracking location by the following procedure. For each location, we estimated the probability that the elephant’s true position was at the nominal position where the bearing lines from the two tracking stations crossed. We also estimated the probability that the true position was at each point on a 54×46 km grid of points spaced at 250-m intervals (corresponding to the study area shown in Fig. 1). These probabilities were estimated based on the error function for bearings obtained from a single tracking station. The error function is approximated by a normal distribution of angular tracking errors with mean of 0° and standard deviation of 1.6°, based on earlier tests of the accuracy of the tracking system (R.B. Martin, unpublished data). We then identified the area needed to encompass 95% of the total cumulative probability around each estimated location, which we call the area of uncertainty (AOU). The most likely location for the elephant is where the bearing lines cross, with the probability falling off with increasing distance from there toward the edges of the AOU. The elephant’s true position is somewhere within the AOU in approximately 95% of all cases. In general, the AOU is small and radially symmetrical where the two bearing lines are perpendicular to each other. As the difference between the two bearings diminishes, the AOU becomes more of an elongated ellipse.
For each location, we estimated the average distance between the elephant’s estimated or nominal position and its true position. This distance is called the mean location error (MLE). The MLE is a weighted average of the distances of all gridpoints within the AOU from the nominal location. The weighting value for each point is the ratio of its probability of being the true location to the probability that the nominal location is correct.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Charif, R.A., Ramey, R.R., Langbauer, W.R. et al. Spatial relationships and matrilineal kinship in African savanna elephant (Loxodonta africana) clans.Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57, 327–338 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-004-0867-5
- Received: 27 May 2004
- Revised: 22 September 2004
- Accepted: 13 October 2004
- Published: 02 December 2004
- Issue Date: February 2005
- DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-004-0867-5