Frank Bernieri - Profile on Academia.edu (original) (raw)
Papers by Frank Bernieri
Intelligence judgments are misleading at first but improve over time
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2014
Measuring Individual Differences With a Wide Scope
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2002
Facial width-to-height ratio predicts testosterone-linked traits and behaviors
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2014
Post hoc scale construction
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2005
Toward a Taxonomy of Interpersonal Sensitivity
Giving off a rosy glow: The experimental manipulation of global optimism
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2005
Interpersonal Rejection, Felt Security and the Desirability of New Relationships
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2006
Toward Predicting the Good Judge: Three Proposed Underlying Moderators
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2003
Human-Robot Interaction, Mar 7, 2016
We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for s... more We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for specifying which objects should be kept private (i.e., not visible) in an office environment. Our study context is a robot "janitor" system that has the ability to blur out specified objects from its video feed. One interface is a traditional point-and-click GUI on a computer monitor, while the other two operate in the real, physical space: users either place markers on the objects to indicate privacy or use a wand tool to point at them. This late-breaking report presents qualitative feedback from users for improving the interfaces.
Evaluation of physical marker interfaces for protecting visual privacy from mobile robots
We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for s... more We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for specifying which objects should be kept private (i.e., not visible) in an office environment. Our study context is a robot “janitor” system that has the ability to blur out specified objects from its video feed. One interface is a traditional point-and-click GUI on a computer monitor, while the other two operate in the real, physical space: users either place markers on the objects to indicate privacy or use a wand tool to point at them. We compare the interfaces using both self-report (e.g., surveys) and behavioral measures. Our results showed that (1) the graphical interface performed better both in terms of time and usability, and (2) using persistent markers increased the participants' ability to recall what they tagged. Choosing the right interface appears to depend on the application scenario. We also summarize feedback from the participants for improving interfaces that specify visual privacy preferences.
CUE Intelligence Poster.pdf
SimS Intelligence Project (CUE).pptx
Intelligence is an illusion: First impressions of intelligence are probably wrong
Personality perception accuracy: A developmental approach
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2002
Perception of Personality: Combining the Criterion Measures of Personality Assessment
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2001
Emotional contagion in conversational language
High self-monitors are "social chameleons" who monitor and control their expressive behavior with... more High self-monitors are "social chameleons" who monitor and control their expressive behavior with the goal of producing favorable impressions on others (Snyder, 1987). Low self-monitors are less motivated by self-presentation goals and exhibit behavioral consistency that more closely reflects their attitudes, internal states, and assessed traits (Snyder, 1987). 1.___ My behavior with my romantic interest is usually an expression of my true inner feelings, attitudes, and beliefs. 2.___While with my romantic interest, I'm not always the person I appear to be. 3.___In order to get along and be liked, I tend to be what my romantic interest expects me to be rather than anything else. 1.___ My behavior at work is usually an expression of my true inner feelings, attitudes, and beliefs. 2.___ While at work, I'm not always the person I appear to be. 3.___ In order to get along and be liked at work I tend to be what people expect me to be rather than anything else.
We Can’t Compare Apples to Oranges: Establishing Validity Between Drastically Different Emotional Intelligence Tests
Original submission was .pub with Microsoft Publisher 2010. File was converted to .pdf using Micr... more Original submission was .pub with Microsoft Publisher 2010. File was converted to .pdf using Microsoft Publisher 2010.
Psychology, 2018
Optimists hold positive expectancies for their future, which some have suggested leads to advanta... more Optimists hold positive expectancies for their future, which some have suggested leads to advantages in the social realm (Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010). Unfortunately, the research supporting this notion is scant and suffers from the confound that self-reports from optimists reflect their optimistic perspective. To address this issue, the present study examined the impact of optimism on interpersonal outcomes assessed from the perspective of those in relationships with each target. We recruited 182 participants to complete a series of psychological measures and interpersonal activities over the course of ten weeks. Participants rated themselves and each other on the five-factor traits at three stages in the developing relationship: zero-acquaintance, after their first conversation with each other, and after nine weeks of acquaintance. Two additional informants nominated by each target as those who knew them well (i.e. friends or family members) provided more extensive personality descriptions using a California Q-Set. Optimists consistently rated themselves as more agreeable and less neurotic than those low in optimism, but only the difference in neuroticism was detectable by perceivers. Furthermore, this difference was discernable only after nine-weeks of acquaintanceship had been established. Target optimism had no impact on first impressions. Although there may exist an optimistic personality profile across the five major traits, we found little evidence to suggest that anything other than lower neuroticism contributes to the impact that optimism might have on one's social life and relationships.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Apr 9, 2017
Who among your colleagues consistently asks you to organize social events though you would much r... more Who among your colleagues consistently asks you to organize social events though you would much rather stay at home curled up with a good book? Who is likely to offer to assist you in performing these tasks? In this report, we have framed the question of trait perception accuracy as the relative ability to discriminate a small group of individuals on the basis of a specific trait. More importantly, we are interested in learning when (at what point in a relationship) this ability to discriminate between others, or accuracy, improves the most. We focused on five major trait domains to uncover which traits can be observed immediately and which traits require an extensive interaction history before they are revealed. We report the trajectories of trait perception accuracy for the five traits assessed at three critical points throughout a developing relationship: (a) zero acquaintance, (b) immediately after completing the first conversation, and finally (c) after becoming well-acquainted with each other 10 weeks later. Investigations of Trait Perception Accuracy at Discrete Phases in a Relationship Up until now, much of the research on trait perception accuracy has been conducted at discrete time points in the acquaintanceship process (e.g., after strangers formulate a first impression, among best friends). One of these time points, known as zero acquaintance, attempts to capture the precise moment before a relationship has begun. This construct has been operationalized in a number of ways, ranging from judgments employing photographs of targets (
Intelligence judgments are misleading at first but improve over time
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2014
Measuring Individual Differences With a Wide Scope
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2002
Facial width-to-height ratio predicts testosterone-linked traits and behaviors
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2014
Post hoc scale construction
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2005
Toward a Taxonomy of Interpersonal Sensitivity
Giving off a rosy glow: The experimental manipulation of global optimism
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2005
Interpersonal Rejection, Felt Security and the Desirability of New Relationships
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2006
Toward Predicting the Good Judge: Three Proposed Underlying Moderators
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2003
Human-Robot Interaction, Mar 7, 2016
We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for s... more We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for specifying which objects should be kept private (i.e., not visible) in an office environment. Our study context is a robot "janitor" system that has the ability to blur out specified objects from its video feed. One interface is a traditional point-and-click GUI on a computer monitor, while the other two operate in the real, physical space: users either place markers on the objects to indicate privacy or use a wand tool to point at them. This late-breaking report presents qualitative feedback from users for improving the interfaces.
Evaluation of physical marker interfaces for protecting visual privacy from mobile robots
We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for s... more We present a study that examines the efficiency and usability of three different interfaces for specifying which objects should be kept private (i.e., not visible) in an office environment. Our study context is a robot “janitor” system that has the ability to blur out specified objects from its video feed. One interface is a traditional point-and-click GUI on a computer monitor, while the other two operate in the real, physical space: users either place markers on the objects to indicate privacy or use a wand tool to point at them. We compare the interfaces using both self-report (e.g., surveys) and behavioral measures. Our results showed that (1) the graphical interface performed better both in terms of time and usability, and (2) using persistent markers increased the participants' ability to recall what they tagged. Choosing the right interface appears to depend on the application scenario. We also summarize feedback from the participants for improving interfaces that specify visual privacy preferences.
CUE Intelligence Poster.pdf
SimS Intelligence Project (CUE).pptx
Intelligence is an illusion: First impressions of intelligence are probably wrong
Personality perception accuracy: A developmental approach
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2002
Perception of Personality: Combining the Criterion Measures of Personality Assessment
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2001
Emotional contagion in conversational language
High self-monitors are "social chameleons" who monitor and control their expressive behavior with... more High self-monitors are "social chameleons" who monitor and control their expressive behavior with the goal of producing favorable impressions on others (Snyder, 1987). Low self-monitors are less motivated by self-presentation goals and exhibit behavioral consistency that more closely reflects their attitudes, internal states, and assessed traits (Snyder, 1987). 1.___ My behavior with my romantic interest is usually an expression of my true inner feelings, attitudes, and beliefs. 2.___While with my romantic interest, I'm not always the person I appear to be. 3.___In order to get along and be liked, I tend to be what my romantic interest expects me to be rather than anything else. 1.___ My behavior at work is usually an expression of my true inner feelings, attitudes, and beliefs. 2.___ While at work, I'm not always the person I appear to be. 3.___ In order to get along and be liked at work I tend to be what people expect me to be rather than anything else.
We Can’t Compare Apples to Oranges: Establishing Validity Between Drastically Different Emotional Intelligence Tests
Original submission was .pub with Microsoft Publisher 2010. File was converted to .pdf using Micr... more Original submission was .pub with Microsoft Publisher 2010. File was converted to .pdf using Microsoft Publisher 2010.
Psychology, 2018
Optimists hold positive expectancies for their future, which some have suggested leads to advanta... more Optimists hold positive expectancies for their future, which some have suggested leads to advantages in the social realm (Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010). Unfortunately, the research supporting this notion is scant and suffers from the confound that self-reports from optimists reflect their optimistic perspective. To address this issue, the present study examined the impact of optimism on interpersonal outcomes assessed from the perspective of those in relationships with each target. We recruited 182 participants to complete a series of psychological measures and interpersonal activities over the course of ten weeks. Participants rated themselves and each other on the five-factor traits at three stages in the developing relationship: zero-acquaintance, after their first conversation with each other, and after nine weeks of acquaintance. Two additional informants nominated by each target as those who knew them well (i.e. friends or family members) provided more extensive personality descriptions using a California Q-Set. Optimists consistently rated themselves as more agreeable and less neurotic than those low in optimism, but only the difference in neuroticism was detectable by perceivers. Furthermore, this difference was discernable only after nine-weeks of acquaintanceship had been established. Target optimism had no impact on first impressions. Although there may exist an optimistic personality profile across the five major traits, we found little evidence to suggest that anything other than lower neuroticism contributes to the impact that optimism might have on one's social life and relationships.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Apr 9, 2017
Who among your colleagues consistently asks you to organize social events though you would much r... more Who among your colleagues consistently asks you to organize social events though you would much rather stay at home curled up with a good book? Who is likely to offer to assist you in performing these tasks? In this report, we have framed the question of trait perception accuracy as the relative ability to discriminate a small group of individuals on the basis of a specific trait. More importantly, we are interested in learning when (at what point in a relationship) this ability to discriminate between others, or accuracy, improves the most. We focused on five major trait domains to uncover which traits can be observed immediately and which traits require an extensive interaction history before they are revealed. We report the trajectories of trait perception accuracy for the five traits assessed at three critical points throughout a developing relationship: (a) zero acquaintance, (b) immediately after completing the first conversation, and finally (c) after becoming well-acquainted with each other 10 weeks later. Investigations of Trait Perception Accuracy at Discrete Phases in a Relationship Up until now, much of the research on trait perception accuracy has been conducted at discrete time points in the acquaintanceship process (e.g., after strangers formulate a first impression, among best friends). One of these time points, known as zero acquaintance, attempts to capture the precise moment before a relationship has begun. This construct has been operationalized in a number of ways, ranging from judgments employing photographs of targets (