The physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan - PubMed (original) (raw)

The physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan

Bum Jin Park et al. Environ Health Prev Med. 2010 Jan.

Abstract

This paper reviews previous research on the physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing), and presents new results from field experiments conducted in 24 forests across Japan. The term Shinrin-yoku was coined by the Japanese Ministry of Agriculture, Forestry, and Fisheries in 1982, and can be defined as making contact with and taking in the atmosphere of the forest. In order to clarify the physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku, we conducted field experiments in 24 forests across Japan. In each experiment, 12 subjects (280 total; ages 21.7 +/- 1.5 year) walked in and viewed a forest or city area. On the first day, six subjects were sent to a forest area, and the others to a city area. On the second day, each group was sent to the other area as a cross-check. Salivary cortisol, blood pressure, pulse rate, and heart rate variability were used as indices. These indices were measured in the morning at the accommodation facility before breakfast and also both before and after the walking (for 16 +/- 5 min) and viewing (for 14 +/- 2 min). The R-R interval was also measured during the walking and viewing periods. The results show that forest environments promote lower concentrations of cortisol, lower pulse rate, lower blood pressure, greater parasympathetic nerve activity, and lower sympathetic nerve activity than do city environments. These results will contribute to the development of a research field dedicated to forest medicine, which may be used as a strategy for preventive medicine.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1

Fig. 1

Forest viewing and walking

Fig. 2

Fig. 2

Change in salivary cortisol concentration after forest viewing and walking. Mean ± standard deviation (SD); ** p < 0.01; _p_-value by t test

Fig. 3

Fig. 3

Change in pulse rate after forest viewing and walking. Mean ± SD; ** p < 0.01; p value by t test

Fig. 4

Fig. 4

Change in systolic blood pressure after forest viewing and walking. Mean ± SD; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05; p value by t test

Fig. 5

Fig. 5

Change in diastolic blood pressure after forest viewing and walking. Mean ± SD; * p < 0.05; p value by t test

Fig. 6

Fig. 6

Change in HF power of HRV upon forest viewing and walking. Mean ± SE; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05; p value by t test

Fig. 7

Fig. 7

Change in LF/HF of HRV upon forest viewing and walking. Mean ± SE; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05; p value by t test

Fig. 8

Fig. 8

Change in average POMS scores upon forest viewing. Mean ± SD; n = 116; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05; p value by Wilcoxon signed-rank test. T–A tension and anxiety, D depression and dejection, A–H anger and hostility, F fatigue, C confusion, V vigor

Fig. 9

Fig. 9

Change in average POMS scores upon forest walking. Mean ± SD; n = 78; ** p < 0.01; p value by Wilcoxon signed-rank test. T–A tension and anxiety, D depression and dejection, A–H anger and hostility, F fatigue, C confusion, and V vigor

Fig. 10

Fig. 10

Relationship between POMS and physical environmental factors in forest area [38]

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-4944(05)80184-7'}\]}
    2. Ulrich RS, Simons RF, Losito BD, Fiorito E, Miles MA, Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Envir Psychol. 1991;11:201–30.
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/S0749-3797(00)00317-2', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-3797(00)00317-2'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11275453', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11275453/'}\]}
    2. Frumkin H. Beyond toxicity: human health and the natural environment. Am J Prev Med. 2001;20(3):234–40. - PubMed
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1038/7299', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1038/7299'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '10204547', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10204547/'}\]}
    2. Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ, Bermudez P, Evans AC. Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:382–7. - PubMed
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00060-9', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1016/s0926-6410(99)00060-9'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '10808134', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10808134/'}\]}
    2. Buchanan TW, Lutz K, Mirzazade S, Specht K, Shah NJ, Zilles K, et al. Recognition of emotional prosody and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study. Cogn Brain Res. 2000;9:227–38. - PubMed
    1. {'text': '', 'ref_index': 1, 'ids': [{'type': 'DOI', 'value': '10.1038/85076', 'is_inner': False, 'url': 'https://doi.org/10.1038/85076'}, {'type': 'PubMed', 'value': '11224538', 'is_inner': True, 'url': 'http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11224538/'}\]}
    2. Goel V, Dolan RJ. The functional anatomy of humor: segregating cognitive and affective components. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:237–8. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources