Centralized sanctioning and legitimate authority promote cooperation in humans - PubMed (original) (raw)
Centralized sanctioning and legitimate authority promote cooperation in humans
Delia Baldassarri et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011.
Abstract
Social sanctioning is widely considered a successful strategy to promote cooperation among humans. In situations in which individual and collective interests are at odds, incentives to free-ride induce individuals to refrain from contributing to public goods provision. Experimental evidence from public goods games shows that when endowed with sanctioning powers, conditional cooperators can discipline defectors, thus leading to greater levels of cooperation. However, extant evidence is based on peer punishment institutions, whereas in complex societies, systems of control are often centralized: for instance, we do not sanction our neighbors for driving too fast, the police do. Here we show the effect of centralized sanctioning and legitimate authority on cooperation. We designed an adaptation of the public goods game in which sanctioning power is given to a single monitor, and we experimentally manipulated the process by which the monitor is chosen. To increase the external validity of the study, we conducted lab-in-the-field experiments involving 1,543 Ugandan farmers from 50 producer cooperatives. This research provides evidence of the effectiveness of centralized sanctioning and demonstrates the causal effect of legitimacy on cooperation: participants are more responsive to the authority of an elected monitor than a randomly chosen monitor. Our essay contributes to the literature on the evolution of cooperation by introducing the idea of role differentiation. In complex societies, cooperative behavior is not only sustained by mechanisms of selection and reciprocity among peers, but also by the legitimacy that certain actors derive from their position in the social hierarchy.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Figures
Fig. 1.
Average contribution to the public good in the baseline (black), random monitor (blue), and elected monitor (red) conditions. For rounds 3 and 6 we report the percentage increase in contributions comparing the random and baseline conditions, and elected with random [e.g., in round 3, subjects in the random monitor contributed 16.6% more (P = 0.000) than in the baseline condition, and subjects in the elected monitor contributed 8.8% more than in the random monitor condition]. n = 1,446 (1,543 players − 97 monitors).
Fig. 2.
The marginal effect of an elected monitor on contributions is twice as big as that of the random monitor, both with respect to players’ expectations and reaction to punishment. Plot of the estimated change in contributions (A) for all players from second preliminary round to round 3; (B) for sanctioned player in rounds 3–6; and (C) for sanctioned players in rounds 3–6 distinguishing between monitors with a dominant or nondominant profile. Parameter estimates come from multilevel models in which we control for individual and group-level predictors (
SI Appendix, Tables S2 and S3
). All continuous variables are held constant at mean values, and the categorical variables are set to male and born local.
Fig. 3.
When facing similar distributions of contributions, elected and random monitors have similar sanctioning behavior. The plots present a comparison of the sanctioning behavior of matched pairs of monitors. We used a Kullback–Leibler divergence measure to match the distribution of contributions that an elected monitor faced with the closest distribution faced by a random monitor. Plot of the (A) number of players sanctioned and (B) maximum contribution sanctioned per round. Vertical bars indicate ±SD. P values from Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test are all greater than conventional levels of significance.
Similar articles
- The Impact of Elections on Cooperation: Evidence from a Lab-in-the-Field Experiment in Uganda.
Grossman G, Baldassarri D. Grossman G, et al. Am J Pol Sci. 2012 Oct 1;56(4):964-985. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2012.00596.x. Am J Pol Sci. 2012. PMID: 23729913 Free PMC article. - Punitive preferences, monetary incentives and tacit coordination in the punishment of defectors promote cooperation in humans.
Diekmann A, Przepiorka W. Diekmann A, et al. Sci Rep. 2015 May 19;5:10321. doi: 10.1038/srep10321. Sci Rep. 2015. PMID: 25988875 Free PMC article. - Can centralized sanctioning promote trust in social dilemmas? A two-level trust game with incomplete information.
Wang RY, Ng CN. Wang RY, et al. PLoS One. 2015 Apr 16;10(4):e0124513. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124513. eCollection 2015. PLoS One. 2015. PMID: 25879752 Free PMC article. - The role of threats in animal cooperation.
Cant MA. Cant MA. Proc Biol Sci. 2011 Jan 22;278(1703):170-8. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1241. Epub 2010 Aug 26. Proc Biol Sci. 2011. PMID: 20798110 Free PMC article. Review. - How small-scale societies achieve large-scale cooperation.
Glowacki L, Lew-Levy S. Glowacki L, et al. Curr Opin Psychol. 2022 Apr;44:44-48. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.08.026. Epub 2021 Aug 28. Curr Opin Psychol. 2022. PMID: 34562700 Review.
Cited by
- The Impact of Elections on Cooperation: Evidence from a Lab-in-the-Field Experiment in Uganda.
Grossman G, Baldassarri D. Grossman G, et al. Am J Pol Sci. 2012 Oct 1;56(4):964-985. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2012.00596.x. Am J Pol Sci. 2012. PMID: 23729913 Free PMC article. - Adaptive and bounded investment returns promote cooperation in spatial public goods games.
Chen X, Liu Y, Zhou Y, Wang L, Perc M. Chen X, et al. PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e36895. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036895. Epub 2012 May 16. PLoS One. 2012. PMID: 22615836 Free PMC article. - Group leaders establish cooperative norms that persist in subsequent interactions.
Harrell A. Harrell A. PLoS One. 2019 Sep 19;14(9):e0222724. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0222724. eCollection 2019. PLoS One. 2019. PMID: 31536555 Free PMC article. - Punishment: one tool, many uses.
Raihani NJ, Bshary R. Raihani NJ, et al. Evol Hum Sci. 2019 Nov 12;1:e12. doi: 10.1017/ehs.2019.12. eCollection 2019. Evol Hum Sci. 2019. PMID: 37588410 Free PMC article. Review. - Discriminatory punishment undermines the enforcement of group cooperation.
Molenmaker WE, Gross J, de Kwaadsteniet EW, van Dijk E, de Dreu CKW. Molenmaker WE, et al. Sci Rep. 2023 Apr 13;13(1):6061. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-33167-2. Sci Rep. 2023. PMID: 37055546 Free PMC article.
References
- Olson M. The Logic of Collective Action: Public Goods and the Theory of Groups. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ Press; 1965.
- Fehr E, Gächter S. Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature. 2002;415:137–140. - PubMed
- Sigmund K. Punish or perish? Retaliation and collaboration among humans. Trends Ecol Evol. 2007;22:593–600. - PubMed
- Henrich J, et al. Costly punishment across human societies. Science. 2006;312:1767–1770. - PubMed
- Rockenbach B, Milinski M. The efficient interaction of indirect reciprocity and costly punishment. Nature. 2006;444:718–723. - PubMed
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources
Miscellaneous