Diversity in pitch perception revealed by task dependence - PubMed (original) (raw)
Diversity in pitch perception revealed by task dependence
Malinda J McPherson et al. Nat Hum Behav. 2018 Jan.
Abstract
Pitch conveys critical information in speech, music, and other natural sounds, and is conventionally defined as the perceptual correlate of a sound's fundamental frequency (F0). Although pitch is widely assumed to be subserved by a single F0 estimation process, real-world pitch tasks vary enormously, raising the possibility of underlying mechanistic diversity. To probe pitch mechanisms we conducted a battery of pitch-related music and speech tasks using conventional harmonic sounds and inharmonic sounds whose frequencies lack a common F0. Some pitch-related abilities - those relying on musical interval or voice recognition - were strongly impaired by inharmonicity, suggesting a reliance on F0. However, other tasks, including those dependent on pitch contours in speech and music, were unaffected by inharmonicity, suggesting a mechanism that tracks the frequency spectrum rather than the F0. The results suggest that pitch perception is mediated by several different mechanisms, only some of which conform to traditional notions of pitch.
Figures
Figure 1. Example harmonic and inharmonic tones
(A) Waveform, power spectrum and autocorrelation for a harmonic complex tone with a fundamental frequency (F0) of 250 Hz. The waveform is periodic (repeating in time), with a period corresponding to one cycle of the F0. The power spectrum contains discrete frequency components (harmonics) that are integer multiples of the F0. The harmonic tone yields an autocorrelation of 1 at a time lag corresponding to its period (1/F0). (B) Waveform, power spectrum and autocorrelation for an inharmonic tone generated by randomly ‘jittering’ the harmonics of the tone in (a). The waveform is aperiodic, and the constituent frequency components are not integer multiples of a common F0 (evident in the irregular spacing in the frequency domain). Such inharmonic tones are thus inconsistent with any single F0. The inharmonic tone exhibits no clear peak in its autocorrelation, indicative of its aperiodicity.
Figure 2. Task, example stimuli, and results for Experiments 1 and 2 – pitch discrimination with pairs of synthetic tones and pairs of instrument notes
(A) Schematic of the trial structure for Experiment 1. During each trial, participants heard two tones and judged whether the second tone was higher or lower than the first tone. (B) Schematic of the three conditions for Experiment 1. Harmonic trials consisted of two harmonic tones. Inharmonic trials contained two inharmonic tones, where each tone was made inharmonic by the same jitter pattern, such that the frequency ratios between components were preserved. This maintains a correspondence in the spectral pattern between the two tones, as for harmonic notes (indicated by red arrows). For Inharmonic-Changing trials, a different jitter pattern was applied to the harmonics of each tone, eliminating the correspondence in the spectral pattern. (C) Power spectra of two examples tones from Experiment 1 (with F0s of 200 and 400 Hz, to convey the range of F0s used in the experiment). The fixed bandpass filter applied to each tone is evident in the envelope of the spectrum, as is the low-pass noise added to mask distortion products. The filter was intended to eliminate the spectral centroid or edge as a cue for pitch changes. (D) Results from Experiment 1. Error bars denote standard error of the mean. (E) Example power spectra of harmonic and inharmonic violin notes from Experiment 2. (F) Results from Experiment 2. Error bars denote standard error of the mean.
Figure 3. Task and results for Experiment 3 – melodic contour discrimination
(A) Schematic of the trial structure for Experiment 3. Participants heard two melodies with note-to-note steps of +1 or -1 semitones, and judged whether the two melodies were the same or different. The second melody was always transposed up in pitch relative to the first melody. (B) Results from Experiment 3. Performance was measured as the area under Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curves. Error bars denote standard error of the mean.
Figure 4. Tasks and results for Experiments 4 and 5 – speech contour perception and Mandarin tone perception
(A) Schematic of the trial structure for Experiment 4. Participants heard three one-second resynthesized speech utterances, the first or last of which had a random frequency modulation (1-2 Hz bandpass noise, with modulation depth varied across conditions) added to the F0 contour. Participants were asked whether the first or last speech excerpt differed from second speech excerpt. The second except was always shifted up in pitch to force listeners to make judgments about the prosodic contour rather than the absolute pitch of the stimuli. (B) Example spectrograms of stimuli from harmonic and inharmonic trials in Experiment 4. Note the even and jittered spacing of frequency components in the two trial types. In these examples, the final excerpt in the trial contains the added frequency modulation. (C) Results from Experiment 4. Error bars denote standard error of the mean. (D) Schematic of trial structure for Experiment 5. Participants (fluent Mandarin speakers) heard a single resynthesized Mandarin word and were asked to type what they heard (in Pinyin, which assigns numbers to the 5 possible tones). Participants could, for example, hear the word wùlǐ, containing tones 4 and 3, and the correct response would be wu4li3. (E) Results for Experiment 5. Error bars denote standard error of the mean.
Figure 5. Task, results, schematic of incorrect interval trials from Experiment 6 – familiar melody recognition
(A) Stimuli and task for Experiment 6. Participants on Amazon Mechanical Turk heard 24 melodies, modified in various ways, and were asked to identify each melody by typing identifying information into a computer interface. Three conditions (Harmonic, Inharmonic, and Inharmonic-Changing) preserved the pitch intervals between notes. Two additional conditions (Incorrect Intervals with harmonic or inharmonic notes) altered each interval between notes but preserved the contour (direction of pitch change between notes). The Rhythm condition preserved the rhythm of the melody, but used a flat pitch contour. (B) Results from Experiment 6. Error bars denote standard deviations calculated via bootstrap.
Figure 6. Task and results for Experiments 7 and 8 – sour note detection and interval pattern discrimination
(A) Sample trial from Experiment 7. Participants judged whether a melody contained a ‘sour’ (out of key) note. (B) Results for Experiment 7. Performance was measured as the area under Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curves. Error bars denote standard error of the mean. (C) Schematic of a sample trial from Experiment 8. Participants judged whether two melodies were the same or different. On ‘different’ trials (pictured) the two melodies had different intervals between notes, but retained the same contour. The second melody was always transposed up in pitch relative to the first. (D) Results for Experiment 8. Performance was measured as the area under Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curves. Error bars denote standard error of the mean.
Figure 7. Task and results for Experiment 9 – pitch discrimination with large pitch intervals
(A) Schematic of trial structure for Experiment 9. During each trial, participants heard two tones and judged whether the second tone was higher or lower than the first tone. The stimuli and task were identical to those of Experiment 1, except larger step sizes were included. (B) Results from Experiment 9. Error bars denote standard error of the mean.
Figure 8. Task and results for Experiments 10a, 10b, and 11 – famous speaker recognition and novel voice discrimination
(A) Description of Experiments 10a and 10b. Participants on Mechanical Turk heard resynthesized excerpts of speech from recordings of celebrities, and were asked to identify each speaker by typing their guesses into an interface. (B) Results from Experiment 10a, with harmonic speech pitch-shifted between -12 and +12 semitones. Here and in (c), error bars plot standard deviations calculated via bootstrap. (C) Results from Experiment 10b. Stimuli in the Whispered condition were resynthesized with simulated breath noise, removing the carrier frequency contours. (D) Schematic of trial structure for Experiment 11. Participants heard three one-second resynthesized speech utterances from unknown speakers, the first or last of which was spoken by a different speaker than the other two. Participants judged which speaker (first or last) only spoke once. (E) Results from Experiment 11. Error bars denote standard error of the mean.
Similar articles
- Relative pitch representations and invariance to timbre.
McPherson MJ, McDermott JH. McPherson MJ, et al. Cognition. 2023 Mar;232:105327. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105327. Epub 2022 Dec 7. Cognition. 2023. PMID: 36495710 Free PMC article. - Electric and acoustic harmonic integration predicts speech-in-noise performance in hybrid cochlear implant users.
Bonnard D, Schwalje A, Gantz B, Choi I. Bonnard D, et al. Hear Res. 2018 Sep;367:223-230. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2018.06.016. Epub 2018 Jun 28. Hear Res. 2018. PMID: 29980380 Free PMC article. - Time-dependent discrimination advantages for harmonic sounds suggest efficient coding for memory.
McPherson MJ, McDermott JH. McPherson MJ, et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020 Dec 15;117(50):32169-32180. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2008956117. Epub 2020 Dec 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020. PMID: 33262275 Free PMC article. - Training of cochlear implant users to improve pitch perception in the presence of competing place cues.
Vandali A, Sly D, Cowan R, van Hoesel R. Vandali A, et al. Ear Hear. 2015 Mar-Apr;36(2):e1-e13. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000109. Ear Hear. 2015. PMID: 25329372 Clinical Trial. - Pitch, harmonicity and concurrent sound segregation: psychoacoustical and neurophysiological findings.
Micheyl C, Oxenham AJ. Micheyl C, et al. Hear Res. 2010 Jul;266(1-2):36-51. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.09.012. Epub 2009 Sep 27. Hear Res. 2010. PMID: 19788920 Free PMC article. Review.
Cited by
- Rabbits use both spectral and temporal cues to discriminate the fundamental frequency of harmonic complexes with missing fundamentals.
Wagner JD, Gelman A, Hancock KE, Chung Y, Delgutte B. Wagner JD, et al. J Neurophysiol. 2022 Jan 1;127(1):290-312. doi: 10.1152/jn.00366.2021. Epub 2021 Dec 8. J Neurophysiol. 2022. PMID: 34879207 Free PMC article. - A Moan of Pleasure Should Be Breathy: The Effect of Voice Quality on the Meaning of Human Nonverbal Vocalizations.
Anikin A. Anikin A. Phonetica. 2020;77(5):327-349. doi: 10.1159/000504855. Epub 2020 Jan 21. Phonetica. 2020. PMID: 31962309 Free PMC article. - An online headphone screening test based on dichotic pitch.
Milne AE, Bianco R, Poole KC, Zhao S, Oxenham AJ, Billig AJ, Chait M. Milne AE, et al. Behav Res Methods. 2021 Aug;53(4):1551-1562. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01514-0. Epub 2020 Dec 9. Behav Res Methods. 2021. PMID: 33300103 Free PMC article. - Auditory Selectivity for Spectral Contrast in Cortical Neurons and Behavior.
So NLT, Edwards JA, Woolley SMN. So NLT, et al. J Neurosci. 2020 Jan 29;40(5):1015-1027. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1200-19.2019. Epub 2019 Dec 11. J Neurosci. 2020. PMID: 31826944 Free PMC article. - Mistuning perception in music is asymmetric and relies on both beats and inharmonicity.
Madsen SMK, Oxenham AJ. Madsen SMK, et al. Commun Psychol. 2024 Oct 2;2(1):91. doi: 10.1038/s44271-024-00141-1. Commun Psychol. 2024. PMID: 39358548 Free PMC article.
References
- Helmholtz HLF. On the sensations of tone. Longmans, Green, and Co; 1875.
- Lord Rayleigh WS. London Macmillan, Repr 1945. Vol. 504. New York: Dover; 1896. Theory of sound.
- Békésy G von. Experiments in hearing. McGraw-Hill; 1960.
- Plack C, Oxenham A, Fay R, Popper A. Pitch: Neural Coding and Perception. Vol. 24. Springer; 2005.
- DeCheveigné A. In: Pitch: Neural Coding and Perception. Plack CJ, Oxenham AJ, Fay R, Popper A, editors. Springer; 2005. pp. 169–233.
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources