Rebecca Treiman | Washington University in St. Louis (original) (raw)

Uploads

Papers by Rebecca Treiman

Research paper thumbnail of WritingDinosaurLarge andMosquitoSmall: Prephonological Spellers’ Use of Semantic Information

Scientific Studies of Reading, 2015

Research paper thumbnail of Preschool Children’s Knowledge of Letter Patterns in Print

Scientific Studies of Reading

Research paper thumbnail of Learning to Write Words

Current Directions in Psychological Science

Learning to produce the written forms of individual words is an important part of writing. In thi... more Learning to produce the written forms of individual words is an important part of writing. In this article, I review research on how children acquire this skill. I begin by discussing young children’s knowledge about the visual appearance of writing and then consider how learners of alphabetic writing systems begin to use letters to symbolize the sounds they hear in words. The English writing system, the focus of this review, is complex. In the final section of the article, I discuss how older children learn about its subtler patterns. Implications of the research for how children learn and for how spelling should be taught are considered.

Research paper thumbnail of What Research Tells Us About Reading Instruction

Psychological Science in the Public Interest

Research paper thumbnail of Learning to Spell Phonologically: Influences of Children’s Own Names

Scientific Studies of Reading

Research paper thumbnail of Phonological and graphotactic influences on spellers’ decisions about consonant doubling

Research paper thumbnail of What methods of scoring young children's spelling best predict later spelling performance?

Journal of Research in Reading

Research paper thumbnail of Teaching and Learning Spelling

Child Development Perspectives

Research paper thumbnail of Correction to: Phonological and graphotactic influences on spellers’ decisions about consonant doubling

Research paper thumbnail of Preschoolers' knowledge about language-specific properties of writing

The British journal of developmental psychology, Jan 25, 2018

According to the differentiation hypothesis, young children's attempts to write show characte... more According to the differentiation hypothesis, young children's attempts to write show characteristics common to all writing systems, such as linearity. Characteristics that are specific to the writing system of the child's culture emerge only later. We tested this hypothesis by presenting adults who knew both Chinese and English with written productions of Chinese and United States 2- to 5-year-olds and asking them to judge the nationality of the writer. Adults performed significantly above the level expected by chance even with the productions of 2- and 3-year-olds, suggesting that knowledge of language-specific characteristics emerges earlier than previously thought. Children appeared to show more language-specific characteristics in their names than in other writings, for adults performed better with children's names than with other items. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? Children's early attempts to write may show general properties...

Research paper thumbnail of Parent–child conversations about literacy: a longitudinal, observational study

Journal of Child Language

Conversations about literacy-related matters with parents can help prepare children for formal li... more Conversations about literacy-related matters with parents can help prepare children for formal literacy instruction. We studied these conversations using data gathered from fifty-six US families as they engaged in daily activities at home. Analyzing conversations when children were aged 1;10, 2;6, 3;6, and 4;2, we found that explicit talk about the elements and processes of reading and writing occurred even when children were less than two years old and became more common as children grew older. The majority of literacy-related conversations included talk about alphabet letters. Literacy-related conversations occurred in a variety of contexts, not only book-reading. There were few differences as a function of family socioeconomic status in the proportion of utterances during the sessions that occurred in literacy-related conversations. At older ages, however, children in families of lower socioeconomic status bore more of the conversational burden than children in families of higher...

Research paper thumbnail of Statistical Learning and Spelling: Older Prephonological Spellers Produce More Wordlike Spellings Than Younger Prephonological Spellers

Child Development

The authors analyzed the spellings of 179 U.S. children (age = 3 years, 2 months-5 years, 6 month... more The authors analyzed the spellings of 179 U.S. children (age = 3 years, 2 months-5 years, 6 months) who were prephonological spellers, in that they wrote using letters that did not reflect the phonemes in the target items. Supporting the idea that children use their statistical learning skills to learn about the outer form of writing before they begin to spell phonologically, older prephonological spellers showed more knowledge about English letter patterns than did younger prephonological spellers. The written productions of older prephonological spellers were rated by adults as more similar to English words than were the productions of younger prephonological spellers. The older children s spellings were also more wordlike on several objective measures, including length, variability of letters within words, and digram frequency.

Research paper thumbnail of Learning to spell: Phonology and beyond

Cognitive Neuropsychology

An understanding of the nature of writing systems and of the typical course of spelling developme... more An understanding of the nature of writing systems and of the typical course of spelling development is an essential foundation for understanding the problems of children who have serious difficulties in learning to spell. The present article seeks to provide that foundation. It argues that the dual-route models of spelling that underlie much existing research and practice are based on overly simple assumptions about how writing systems work and about how spelling skills develop. Many writing systems include not only context-free links from phonemes to letters but also context-sensitive phonological patterns, morphological influences, and graphotactic patterns. According to an alternative framework, IMP (integration of multiple patterns), spellers acquire multiple sources of information through use of their statistical-learning skills and through direct instruction. Children learn the spelling of a word most easily when different patterns converge on the spelling, and they have difficulty when patterns conflict. Implications of these ideas for assessment and instruction are considered.

Research paper thumbnail of Differentiation of writing and drawing by U.S. two- to five-year-olds

Cognitive Development

To investigate preschoolers' knowledge about symbol systems, we compared the writ... more To investigate preschoolers' knowledge about symbol systems, we compared the written and drawn productions of 2-5-year-old U.S. children. In Study 1, children (N = 88) wrote and drew four targets, including their own name and a picture of themselves. Children differentiated writings from drawings in the implements they used, the size of their productions, and use of recognizable letters. Some distinctions were present in the youngest children and others became more prominent with age. In Study 2, adults (N = 16) who judged whether the productions were writings or drawings performed above the level of chance for all age groups. Adults did better for children's names and self-portraits than for other targets, suggesting that the name plays a leading role in U.S. children's learning about writing. Overall, the results show that children begin to learn about formal differences between writing and drawing at an early age.

Research paper thumbnail of Writing Systems and Spelling Development

This chapter differs from most of its companions in addressing literacy from the stand-point of t... more This chapter differs from most of its companions in addressing literacy from the stand-point of the writer rather than the reader. Literacy research has concentrated on reading, but without the ability to write a person could scarcely be called literate. A full under-standing of ...

Research paper thumbnail of Measures of Kindergarten Spelling and Their Relations to Later Spelling Performance

Scientific Studies of Reading, 2016

Learning the orthographic forms of words is important for both spelling and reading. To determine... more Learning the orthographic forms of words is important for both spelling and reading. To determine whether some methods of scoring children's early spellings predict later spelling performance better than do other methods, we analyzed data from 374 U.S. and Australian children who took a 10-word spelling test at the end of kindergarten (mean age 6 years, 2 months) and a standardized spelling test approximately two years later. Surprisingly, scoring methods that took account of phonological plausibility did not outperform methods that were based only on orthographic correctness. The scoring method that is most widely used in research with young children, which allots a certain number of points to each word and which considers both orthographic and phonological plausibility, did not rise to the top as a predictor. Prediction of Grade 2 spelling performance was improved to a small extent by considering children's tendency to reverse letters in kindergarten.

Research paper thumbnail of Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: their use by children and ad

Research paper thumbnail of Subsyllabic units in computerized reading instruction: Onset-rime vs. Postvowel segmentation*1

J Exp Child Psychol, 1990

Previous studies suggest that adults and children divide spoken syllables into subsyllabic onset-... more Previous studies suggest that adults and children divide spoken syllables into subsyllabic onset-rime units more readily than into any other kind of subsyllabic unit. We asked whether this same onset-rime segmentation might also be beneficial in teaching children to read. That is, can children learn more words segmented at the onset-rime boundary (e.g., CL-AP, D-ISH) than words segmented after the vowel (CLA-P, DI-SH)? In three experiments, first-grade students studied single words presented by a computer connected to a high-quality speech synthesizer. Experiment 1 used words of four letters but only three phonemes apiece (e.g., WHIP, DISH). In some of these words the onset-rime segmentation corresponded to the initial bigram (e.g., WH-IP); in some it did not (e.g., D-ISH). Experiments 2 and 3 used words of four letters and four phonemes (e.g., CLAP, CORN). In all three experiments, onset-rime segmentation proved more helpful than postvowel segmentation in short-term learning of the words.

Research paper thumbnail of Classification of Stops after /s/ by Children and Adults

Research paper thumbnail of Writing systems: Their properties and implications for reading

ABSTRACT An understanding of the nature of writing is an important foundation for studies of how ... more ABSTRACT An understanding of the nature of writing is an important foundation for studies of how people read and how they learn to read. This chapter discusses the characteristics of modern writing systems with a view toward providing that foundation. We consider both the appearance of writing systems and how they function. All writing represents the words of a language according to a set of rules. However, important properties of a language often go unrepresented in writing. Change and variation in the spoken language result in complex links to speech. Redundancies in language and writing mean that readers can often get by without taking in all of the visual information. These redundancies also mean that readers must often supplement the visual information that they do take in with knowledge about the language and about the world.

Research paper thumbnail of WritingDinosaurLarge andMosquitoSmall: Prephonological Spellers’ Use of Semantic Information

Scientific Studies of Reading, 2015

Research paper thumbnail of Preschool Children’s Knowledge of Letter Patterns in Print

Scientific Studies of Reading

Research paper thumbnail of Learning to Write Words

Current Directions in Psychological Science

Learning to produce the written forms of individual words is an important part of writing. In thi... more Learning to produce the written forms of individual words is an important part of writing. In this article, I review research on how children acquire this skill. I begin by discussing young children’s knowledge about the visual appearance of writing and then consider how learners of alphabetic writing systems begin to use letters to symbolize the sounds they hear in words. The English writing system, the focus of this review, is complex. In the final section of the article, I discuss how older children learn about its subtler patterns. Implications of the research for how children learn and for how spelling should be taught are considered.

Research paper thumbnail of What Research Tells Us About Reading Instruction

Psychological Science in the Public Interest

Research paper thumbnail of Learning to Spell Phonologically: Influences of Children’s Own Names

Scientific Studies of Reading

Research paper thumbnail of Phonological and graphotactic influences on spellers’ decisions about consonant doubling

Research paper thumbnail of What methods of scoring young children's spelling best predict later spelling performance?

Journal of Research in Reading

Research paper thumbnail of Teaching and Learning Spelling

Child Development Perspectives

Research paper thumbnail of Correction to: Phonological and graphotactic influences on spellers’ decisions about consonant doubling

Research paper thumbnail of Preschoolers' knowledge about language-specific properties of writing

The British journal of developmental psychology, Jan 25, 2018

According to the differentiation hypothesis, young children's attempts to write show characte... more According to the differentiation hypothesis, young children's attempts to write show characteristics common to all writing systems, such as linearity. Characteristics that are specific to the writing system of the child's culture emerge only later. We tested this hypothesis by presenting adults who knew both Chinese and English with written productions of Chinese and United States 2- to 5-year-olds and asking them to judge the nationality of the writer. Adults performed significantly above the level expected by chance even with the productions of 2- and 3-year-olds, suggesting that knowledge of language-specific characteristics emerges earlier than previously thought. Children appeared to show more language-specific characteristics in their names than in other writings, for adults performed better with children's names than with other items. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? Children's early attempts to write may show general properties...

Research paper thumbnail of Parent–child conversations about literacy: a longitudinal, observational study

Journal of Child Language

Conversations about literacy-related matters with parents can help prepare children for formal li... more Conversations about literacy-related matters with parents can help prepare children for formal literacy instruction. We studied these conversations using data gathered from fifty-six US families as they engaged in daily activities at home. Analyzing conversations when children were aged 1;10, 2;6, 3;6, and 4;2, we found that explicit talk about the elements and processes of reading and writing occurred even when children were less than two years old and became more common as children grew older. The majority of literacy-related conversations included talk about alphabet letters. Literacy-related conversations occurred in a variety of contexts, not only book-reading. There were few differences as a function of family socioeconomic status in the proportion of utterances during the sessions that occurred in literacy-related conversations. At older ages, however, children in families of lower socioeconomic status bore more of the conversational burden than children in families of higher...

Research paper thumbnail of Statistical Learning and Spelling: Older Prephonological Spellers Produce More Wordlike Spellings Than Younger Prephonological Spellers

Child Development

The authors analyzed the spellings of 179 U.S. children (age = 3 years, 2 months-5 years, 6 month... more The authors analyzed the spellings of 179 U.S. children (age = 3 years, 2 months-5 years, 6 months) who were prephonological spellers, in that they wrote using letters that did not reflect the phonemes in the target items. Supporting the idea that children use their statistical learning skills to learn about the outer form of writing before they begin to spell phonologically, older prephonological spellers showed more knowledge about English letter patterns than did younger prephonological spellers. The written productions of older prephonological spellers were rated by adults as more similar to English words than were the productions of younger prephonological spellers. The older children s spellings were also more wordlike on several objective measures, including length, variability of letters within words, and digram frequency.

Research paper thumbnail of Learning to spell: Phonology and beyond

Cognitive Neuropsychology

An understanding of the nature of writing systems and of the typical course of spelling developme... more An understanding of the nature of writing systems and of the typical course of spelling development is an essential foundation for understanding the problems of children who have serious difficulties in learning to spell. The present article seeks to provide that foundation. It argues that the dual-route models of spelling that underlie much existing research and practice are based on overly simple assumptions about how writing systems work and about how spelling skills develop. Many writing systems include not only context-free links from phonemes to letters but also context-sensitive phonological patterns, morphological influences, and graphotactic patterns. According to an alternative framework, IMP (integration of multiple patterns), spellers acquire multiple sources of information through use of their statistical-learning skills and through direct instruction. Children learn the spelling of a word most easily when different patterns converge on the spelling, and they have difficulty when patterns conflict. Implications of these ideas for assessment and instruction are considered.

Research paper thumbnail of Differentiation of writing and drawing by U.S. two- to five-year-olds

Cognitive Development

To investigate preschoolers' knowledge about symbol systems, we compared the writ... more To investigate preschoolers' knowledge about symbol systems, we compared the written and drawn productions of 2-5-year-old U.S. children. In Study 1, children (N = 88) wrote and drew four targets, including their own name and a picture of themselves. Children differentiated writings from drawings in the implements they used, the size of their productions, and use of recognizable letters. Some distinctions were present in the youngest children and others became more prominent with age. In Study 2, adults (N = 16) who judged whether the productions were writings or drawings performed above the level of chance for all age groups. Adults did better for children's names and self-portraits than for other targets, suggesting that the name plays a leading role in U.S. children's learning about writing. Overall, the results show that children begin to learn about formal differences between writing and drawing at an early age.

Research paper thumbnail of Writing Systems and Spelling Development

This chapter differs from most of its companions in addressing literacy from the stand-point of t... more This chapter differs from most of its companions in addressing literacy from the stand-point of the writer rather than the reader. Literacy research has concentrated on reading, but without the ability to write a person could scarcely be called literate. A full under-standing of ...

Research paper thumbnail of Measures of Kindergarten Spelling and Their Relations to Later Spelling Performance

Scientific Studies of Reading, 2016

Learning the orthographic forms of words is important for both spelling and reading. To determine... more Learning the orthographic forms of words is important for both spelling and reading. To determine whether some methods of scoring children's early spellings predict later spelling performance better than do other methods, we analyzed data from 374 U.S. and Australian children who took a 10-word spelling test at the end of kindergarten (mean age 6 years, 2 months) and a standardized spelling test approximately two years later. Surprisingly, scoring methods that took account of phonological plausibility did not outperform methods that were based only on orthographic correctness. The scoring method that is most widely used in research with young children, which allots a certain number of points to each word and which considers both orthographic and phonological plausibility, did not rise to the top as a predictor. Prediction of Grade 2 spelling performance was improved to a small extent by considering children's tendency to reverse letters in kindergarten.

Research paper thumbnail of Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: their use by children and ad

Research paper thumbnail of Subsyllabic units in computerized reading instruction: Onset-rime vs. Postvowel segmentation*1

J Exp Child Psychol, 1990

Previous studies suggest that adults and children divide spoken syllables into subsyllabic onset-... more Previous studies suggest that adults and children divide spoken syllables into subsyllabic onset-rime units more readily than into any other kind of subsyllabic unit. We asked whether this same onset-rime segmentation might also be beneficial in teaching children to read. That is, can children learn more words segmented at the onset-rime boundary (e.g., CL-AP, D-ISH) than words segmented after the vowel (CLA-P, DI-SH)? In three experiments, first-grade students studied single words presented by a computer connected to a high-quality speech synthesizer. Experiment 1 used words of four letters but only three phonemes apiece (e.g., WHIP, DISH). In some of these words the onset-rime segmentation corresponded to the initial bigram (e.g., WH-IP); in some it did not (e.g., D-ISH). Experiments 2 and 3 used words of four letters and four phonemes (e.g., CLAP, CORN). In all three experiments, onset-rime segmentation proved more helpful than postvowel segmentation in short-term learning of the words.

Research paper thumbnail of Classification of Stops after /s/ by Children and Adults

Research paper thumbnail of Writing systems: Their properties and implications for reading

ABSTRACT An understanding of the nature of writing is an important foundation for studies of how ... more ABSTRACT An understanding of the nature of writing is an important foundation for studies of how people read and how they learn to read. This chapter discusses the characteristics of modern writing systems with a view toward providing that foundation. We consider both the appearance of writing systems and how they function. All writing represents the words of a language according to a set of rules. However, important properties of a language often go unrepresented in writing. Change and variation in the spoken language result in complex links to speech. Redundancies in language and writing mean that readers can often get by without taking in all of the visual information. These redundancies also mean that readers must often supplement the visual information that they do take in with knowledge about the language and about the world.