The interactive effect of empathy and motor cortex stimulation on hand gesture comprehension (original) (raw)
Abstract
Previous research has demonstrated that the Action-Observation Network (AON) is involved in both emotionalembodiment (empathy) and action-embodiment mechanisms. In the current study, we hypothesized that interfering with the AON will impair action recognition and that this impairment will be modulated by empathy levels. Fifty-two participants conducted a semantic decision task of hand gesture recognition, while we interfered with the AON by applying active (n ¼ 26) or sham (n ¼ 26) transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) to the hand area of the primary motor cortex. We found that interfering with the AON impaired the performance of participants with high empathy levels and enhanced the performance of participants with low empathy. This finding suggests that the embodiment module may be flexible, and that it can be enhanced in individuals with low empathy by simple manipulation of motor activation.
FAQs
AI
What is the effect of tDCS on hand gesture comprehension in different empathy levels?add
The study reveals that tDCS enhances hand gesture comprehension in low empathy individuals but impairs it in high empathy individuals.
How does the Action Observation Network relate to empathy and motor cortex stimulation?add
The findings indicate that the Action Observation Network's activity during motor cortex stimulation varies with empathy levels, affecting hand gesture interpretation.
What mechanisms improve comprehension in low-empathy participants under motor cortex stimulation?add
The paper demonstrates that stimulating the motor cortex enhances feedback from the body to the brain, facilitating the embodiment mechanism in low empathy individuals.
Which specific brain regions are involved in experience sharing and gesture comprehension?add
Research identifies the Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Insula as key regions in experience sharing, relevant for gesture comprehension.
What experimental methods were used to assess empathy's role in gesture comprehension?add
The study utilized tDCS alongside behavioral tasks and an empathy questionnaire to evaluate the effects of empathy on gesture comprehension.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
References (77)
- Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder, 2014. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00791.
- Avenanti, A., Bueti, D., Galati, G., Aglioti, S.M., 2005. Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nat. Neurosci. 8 (7), 955-960. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1481.
- Aziz-Zadeh, L., Sheng, T., Gheytanchi, A., 2010. Common premotor regions for the perception and production of prosody and correlations with empathy and prosodic ability. PloS One 5 (1), e8759. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008759.
- Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., 2004. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34 (2), 163-175. https://doi.org/10.1023/B: JADD.0000022607.19833.00.
- Barsalou, L.W., 2008. Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59 (1), 617-645. https:// doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639.
- Barsalou, L.W., Niedenthal, P.M., Barbey, A.K., Ruppert, J.A., 2003. Social embodiment. In: Psychology of Learning and Motivation -Advances in Research and Theory, vol. 43, pp. 43-92. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(03)01011-9.
- Bastiaansen, J.A.C.J., Thioux, M., Keysers, C., 2009. Evidence for mirror systems in emotions. Phil. Trans. Biol. Sci. 364 (1528), 2391-2404. https://doi.org/10.1098/ rstb.2009.0058.
- Batchelder, L., Brosnan, M., Ashwin, C., 2017. The development and validation of the empathy components questionnaire (ECQ). PloS One 12 (1), e0169185. https://doi. org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169185.
- Bonfils, K.A., Lysaker, P.H., Minor, K.S., Salyers, M.P., 2016. Affective empathy in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 175 (1), 109-117. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.schres.2016.03.037.
- Bortoletto, M., Pellicciari, M.C., Rodella, C., Miniussi, C., 2015. The interaction with task-induced activity is more important than polarization: a tDCS study. Brain Stimulation 8 (2), 269-276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2014.11.006.
- Chartrand, T.L., Bargh, J.A., 1999. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76 (6), 893-910. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893.
- Chiavarino, C., Bugiani, S., Grandi, E., Colle, L., 2013. Is automatic imitation based on goal coding or movement coding? A comparison of goal-directed and goal-less actions. Exp. Psychol. 60 (3), 213-225. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/ a000190.
- Classen, J., Knorr, U., Werhahn, K.J., Schlaug, G., Kunesch, E., Cohen, L.G., et al., 1998. Multimodal output mapping of human central motor representation on different spatial scales. J. Physiol. 512 (1), 163-179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469- 7793.1998.163bf.x.
- Cohen-Maximov, T., Avirame, K., Fl€ oel, A., Lavidor, M., 2015. Modulation of gestural- verbal semantic integration by tDCS. Brain Stimulation 8 (3), 493-498. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.001.
- de Guzman, M., Bird, G., Banissy, M.J., Catmur, C., 2016. Self-other control processes in social cognition: from imitation to empathy. Phil. Trans. Biol. Sci. 371 (1686) https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0079.
- Decety, J., 2010. To what extent is the experience of empathy mediated by shared neural circuits? Emotion Review 2 (3), 204-207. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1754073910361981.
- Decety, J., Jackson, P.L., 2004. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav. Cognit. Neurosci. Rev. 3 (2), 71-100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187.
- Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., Elmehed, K., 2000. Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 11 (1), 86-89. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467- 9280.00221.
- Drimalla, H., Landwehr, N., Hess, U., Dziobek, I., 2019. From face to face: the contribution of facial mimicry to cognitive and emotional empathy. Cognit. Emot. 1-15 https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1596068.
- Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H.R., Wolf, O.T., Convit, A., 2008. Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38 (3), 464-473. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x.
- Ekman, P., Friesen, W.V., 1978. Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA.
- Erdfelder, E., FAul, F., Buchner, A., Lang, A.G., 2009. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41 (4), 1149-1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.
- Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., Rizzolatti, G., 1995. Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurophysiol. 73 (6), 2608-2611. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2608.
- Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.G., Buchner, A., 2007. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39 (2), 175-191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146.
- Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A., Th� eoret, H., 2008. Psychopathy and the mirror neuron system: preliminary findings from a non-psychiatric sample. Psychiatr. Res. 160 (2), 137-144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.08.022.
- Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P.F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., Rizzolotti, G., 2005. Neuroscience: parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science 308 (5722), 662-667. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106138.
- Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., Keysers, C., 2006. Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr. Biol. 16 (18), 1824-1829. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.cub.2006.07.072.
- Hamilton, A.F.d.C., Brindley, R.M., Frith, U., 2007. Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: how valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia 45 (8), 1859-1868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2006.11.022.
- Hawk, S.T., Fischer, A.H., Van Kleef, G.A., 2011. Taking your place or matching your face: two paths to empathic embarrassment. Emotion 11 (3), 502-513. https://doi. org/10.1037/a0022762.
- Heimann, K., Umilta, M.A., Gallese, V., 2013. How the motor-cortex distinguishes among letters, unknown symbols and scribbles. A high density EEG study. Neuropsychologia 51 (13), 2833-2840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.07.014.
- Hess, U., Fischer, A., 2013. Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 17 (2), 142-157. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312472607.
- Hickok, G., 2014. The Myth of Mirror Neurons: the Real Neuroscience of Communication and Cognition. W. W. Norton & Company.
- Hsu, C.-T., Sims, T., Chakrabarti, B., 2018. How mimicry influences the neural correlates of reward: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 116, 61-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2017.08.018.
- Iacoboni, M., 2009. Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60 (1), 653-670. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604.
- Jabbi, M., Keysers, C., 2008. Inferior frontal gyrus activity triggers anterior insula response to emotional facial expressions. Emotion 8 (6), 775-780. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/a0014194.
- Janssen, A.M., Oostendorp, T.F., Stegeman, D.F., 2015. The coil orientation dependency of the electric field induced by TMS for M1 and other brain areas. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 12 (1), 47. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-015-0036-2.
- Jospe, K., Fl€ oe, A., Lavidor, M., 2018. The interaction between embodiment and empathy in facial expression recognition. Soc. Cognit. Affect Neurosci. 13 (2) https://doi.org/ 10.1093/scan/nsy005.
- Jospe, K., Fl€ oel, A., Lavidor, M., 2017. The role of embodiment and individual empathy levels in gesture comprehension. Exp. Psychol. 64 (1) https://doi.org/10.1027/ 1618-3169/a000351.
- Kaplan, J.T., Iacoboni, M., 2006. Getting a grip on other minds: mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy. Soc. Neurosci. 1 (3-4), 175-183. https://doi. org/10.1080/17470910600985605.
- Kelly, S.D., Barr, D.J., Church, R.B., Lynch, K., 1999. Offering a hand to pragmatic understanding: the role of speech and gesture in comprehension and memory. J. Mem. Lang. 40 (4), 577-592. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2634.
- Keysers, C., Gazzola, V., 2009. Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19 (6), 666-671. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.006.
- Keysers, C., Kaas, J.H., Gazzola, V., 2010. Somatosensation in social perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11 (6), 417-428. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2833.
- Knafo, A., Zahn-Waxler, C., Van Hulle, C., Robinson, J.L., Rhee, S.H., 2008. The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion 8 (6), 737. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014179.
- Kochiyama, T., Kubota, Y., Sato, W., Toichi, M., Sawada, R., Uono, S., et al., 2017. Neural substrates of the ability to recognize facial expressions: a voxel-based morphometry study. Soc. Cognit. Affect Neurosci. 12 (3), 487-495. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/ nsw142.
- Koehne, S., Hatri, A., Cacioppo, J.T., Dziobek, I., 2016. Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition 146, 8-15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.007.
- Koster-Hale, J., Richardson, H., Velez, N., Asaba, M., Young, L., Saxe, R., 2017. Mentalizing regions represent distributed, continuous, and abstract dimensions of others' beliefs. Neuroimage 161, 9-18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2017.08.026.
- Lamm, C., Decety, J., Singer, T., 2011. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54 (3), 2492-2502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2010.10.014.
- Levy, J., Goldstein, A., Influs, M., Masalha, S., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Feldman, R., 2016. Adolescents growing up amidst intractable conflict attenuate brain response to pain of outgroup. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. 113 (48), 13696-13701. https:// doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1612903113.
- Lombardo, M.V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E.T., Baron-Cohen, S., 2011. Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism. Neuroimage 56 (3), 1832-1838. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2011.02.067.
- Morelli, S.A., Lieberman, M.D., Zaki, J., 2015. The emerging study of positive empathy. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 9 (2), 57-68. https://doi.org/10.1111/ spc3.12157.
- Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A.D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., Fried, I., 2010. Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Curr. Biol. 20 (8), 750-756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045.
- Murata, A., Saito, H., Schug, J., Ogawa, K., Kameda, T., 2016. Spontaneous facial mimicry is enhanced by the goal of inferring emotional states: evidence for moderation of "automatic" mimicry by higher cognitive processes. PloS One 11 (4), e0153128. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153128.
- Niedenthal, P.M., 2007. Embodying emotion. Science 316 (5827), 1002-1005. https:// doi.org/10.1126/science.1136930.
- Nitsche, M.A., Paulus, W., 2000. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527 (3), 633-639. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x.
- Nitsche, M.A., Paulus, W., 2001. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57 (10), 1899-1901. https://doi. org/10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899.
- Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9 (1), 97-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932 (71)90067-4.
- Perry, A., Saunders, S.N., Stiso, J., Dewar, C., Lubell, J., Meling, T.R., et al., 2017. Effects of prefrontal cortex damage on emotion understanding: EEG and behavioural evidence. Brain 140 (4), 1086-1099. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awx031.
- Perry, A., Troje, N.F., Bentin, S., 2010. Exploring motor system contributions to the perception of social information: evidence from EEG activity in the mu/alpha frequency range. Soc. Neurosci. 5 (3), 272-284. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 17470910903395767.
- Pineda, J.A., 2005. The functional significance of mu rhythms: translating "seeing" and "hearing" into "doing. Brain Res. Rev. 50 (1), 57-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. brainresrev.2005.04.005.
- Pulvermüller, F., H€ arle, M., Hummel, F., 2001. Walking or talking?: behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing. Brain Lang. 78 (2), 143-168. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2000.2390.
- Richter, D., Kunzmann, U., 2011. Age differences in three facets of empathy: performance-based evidence. Psychol. Aging 26 (1), 60-70. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/a0021138.
- Rizzolatti, G., Scandolara, C., Matelli, M., Gentilucci, M., 1981. Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses. Behav. Brain Res. 2 (2), 147-163. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X.
- Rizzolatti, G., Sinigaglia, C., 2010. The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11 (4), 264-274. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2805.
- Rychlowska, M., Cañadas, E., Wood, A., Krumhuber, E.G., Fischer, A., Niedenthal, P.M., 2014. Blocking mimicry makes true and false smiles look the same. PloS One 9 (3), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090876.
- Saxe, R., Kanwisher, N., 2003. People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in "theory of mind. Neuroimage 19 (4), 1835-1842. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1.
- Saxe, R., Moran, J.M., Scholz, J., Gabrieli, J., 2006. Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects. Soc. Cognit. Affect Neurosci. 1 (3), 229-234. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl034.
- Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., 2011. The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 17 (1), 18-24. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858410379268.
- Strack, F., Martin, L.L., Stepper, S., 1988. Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54 (5), 768-777. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768.
- Sze, J.A., Gyurak, A., Goodkind, M.S., Levenson, R.W., 2012. Greater emotional empathy and prosocial behavior in late life. Emotion 12 (5), 1129-1140. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/a0025011.
- Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M.C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., et al., 2005. Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 17 (2), 273-281. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053124965.
- Thomas, R., Sink, J., Haggard, P., 2013. Sensory effects of action observation: evidence for perceptual enhancement driven by sensory rather than motor simulation. Exp. Psychol. 60 (5), 335-346. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000203.
- Tseng, L.Y., Tseng, P., Liang, W.K., Hung, D.L., Tzeng, O.J.L., Muggleton, N.G., Juan, C. H., 2014. The role of superior temporal sulcus in the control of irrelevant emotional face processing: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. Neuropsychologia 64, 124-133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.015.
- Watkins, K.E., Strafella, A.P., Paus, T., 2003. Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia 41 (8), 989-994. https:// doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00316-0.
- Weiss, P.H., Ubben, S.D., Kaesberg, S., Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., Liebig, T., Fink, G.R., 2016. Where language meets meaningful action: a combined behavior and lesion analysis of aphasia and apraxia. Brain Struct. Funct. 221 (1), 563-576. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s00429-014-0925-3.
- Williams, J.H.G., Nicolson, A.T.A., Clephan, K.J., de Grauw, H., Perrett, D.I., 2013. A novel method testing the ability to imitate composite emotional expressions reveals an association with empathy. PloS One 8 (4), e61941. https://doi.org/ 10.1371/journal.pone.0061941.
- Zahavi, D., 2010. Empathy, embodiment and interpersonal understanding: from lipps to schutz. Inquiry 53 (3), 285-306. https://doi.org/10.1080/00201741003784663.
- Zaki, J., Weber, J., Bolger, N., Ochsner, K., 2009. The neural bases of empathic accuracy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am. 106 (27), 11382-11387. https://doi.org/ 10.1073/pnas.0902666106.