Development of Listenership for Indian Hindustani Music By Using Mimetic Comprehension (original) (raw)

What You Hear Isn't What You See: The representation and cognition of fast movements in Hindustani music

Meer, Wim van der and Suvarnalata Rao, What You Hear Isn't What You See: The representation and cognition of fast movements in Hindustani music, FRSM 2006, Lucknow

In Hindustani music the space ‘between the notes’ is often more important than the discrete notes themselves. With the help of melography, and more in particular the use of advanced models of pitch perception in computer software, we can actually ‘see’ the precise forms of meend and other aspects of pitch bending. Generally, both musicians and musicologists agree that this graphic representation does much better justice to the music than staff notation or sargam notation. However, there are also serious limitations, especially when we look closely at rapid ornamentations or tans. From perception studies of the seventies it is known that the ear starts averaging out rapidly oscillating pitches at speeds of 6 movements per second. In this paper, we demonstrate the limitation of this hypothesis and attempt to formulate a model of how the visual representation corresponds to the perceived note patterns in case of fast movements involving murki and gamak. The article is online here: http://bakesociety.net/van-der-meer-and-rao-what-you-hear-isnt-what-you-see/

Familiarity overrides complexity in rhythm perception: A cross-cultural comparison of American and Turkish listeners

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012

Despite the ubiquity of dancing and synchronized movement to music, relatively few studies have examined cognitive representations of musical rhythm and meter among listeners from contrasting cultures. We aimed to disentangle the contributions of culture-general and culture-specific influences by examining American and Turkish listeners' detection of temporal disruptions (varying in size from 50-250 ms in duration) to three types of stimuli: simple rhythms found in both American and Turkish music, complex rhythms found only in Turkish music, and highly complex rhythms that are rare in all cultures. Americans were most accurate when detecting disruptions to the simple rhythm. However, they performed less accurately but comparably in both the complex and highly complex conditions. By contrast, Turkish participants performed accurately and indistinguishably in both simple and complex conditions. However, they performed less accurately in the unfamiliar, highly complex condition. Together, these experiments implicate a crucial role of culture-specific listening experience and acquired musical knowledge in rhythmic pattern perception.

Cross-cultural influences on rhythm processing: reproduction, discrimination, and beat tapping

The structures of musical rhythm differ between cultures, despite the fact that the ability to entrain movement to musical rhythm occurs in virtually all individuals across cultures. To measure the influence of culture on rhythm processing, we tested East African and North American adults on perception, production, and beat tapping for rhythms derived from East African and Western music. To assess rhythm perception, participants identified whether pairs of rhythms were the same or different. To assess rhythm production, participants reproduced rhythms after hearing them. To assess beat tapping, participants tapped the beat along with repeated rhythms. We expected that performance in all three tasks would be influenced by the culture of the participant and the culture of the rhythm. Specifically, we predicted that a participant's ability to discriminate, reproduce, and accurately tap the beat would be better for rhythms from their own culture than for rhythms from another culture. In the rhythm discrimination task, there were no differences in discriminating culturally familiar and unfamiliar rhythms. In the rhythm reproduction task, both groups reproduced East African rhythms more accurately than Western rhythms, but East African participants also showed an effect of cultural familiarity, leading to a significant interaction. In the beat tapping task, participants in both groups tapped the beat more accurately for culturally familiar than for unfamiliar rhythms. Moreover, there were differences between the two participant groups, and between the two types of rhythms, in the metrical level selected for beat tapping. The results demonstrate that culture does influence the processing of musical rhythm. In terms of the function of musical rhythm, our results are consistent with theories that musical rhythm enables synchronization. Musical rhythm may foster musical cultural identity by enabling within-group synchronization to music, perhaps supporting social cohesion.

Cultural Factors in Responses to Rhythmic Stimuli

In recent decades the cognitive sciences have seen some remarkable developments that reflect shifting and broadening perspectives of the discipline. One is the recognition that cognition is shaped and formed by an intricate interplay of biological, environmental and experiential, i.e. cultural factors. This realization has profited from a notable tradition of cultural and cross-cultural research in psychology with an initial research emphasis on social cognition, personality traits, cognitive styles, etc. It has produced multiple lines of evidence for cultural differences engendering differences in psychological behavior Kitayama & Cohen, 2007), and, following the introduction of modern imaging methods (fMRI, EEG,MEG), has uncovered influences of cultural factors on various brain functions and activations of specific neural substrates, including low-level perceptual processes .

Music Perception and Cognition: A Review of Recent Cross-Cultural Research

Experimental investigations of cross-cultural music perception and cognition reported during the past decade are described. As globalization and Western music homogenize the world musical environment, it is imperative that diverse music and musical contexts are documented. Processes of music perception include grouping and segmentation, statistical learning and sensitivity to tonal and temporal hierarchies, and the development of tonal and temporal expectations. The interplay of auditory, visual, and motor modalities is discussed in light of synchronization and the way music moves via emotional response. Further research is needed to test deep-rooted psychological assumptions about music cognition with diverse materials and groups in dynamic contexts. Although empirical musicology provides keystones to unlock musical structures and organization, the psychological reality of those theorized structures for listeners and performers, and the broader implications for theories of music perception and cognition, awaits investigation.

Implicit learning of Indian music by Westerners

Studies by Bigand and Barrouillet (1996), Perruchet, Bigand, and Benoit-Gonin (1997), Bigand, Perruchet, and Boyer (1998), and Tillmann, Bharucha, and Bigand, E. (2000) show that short exposure to an artificial grammar trains listeners to distinguish between stimuli that obey and disobey the rules. The present study considers the extent to which rules of a musical tradition with which subjects have had no contact are learned. Within Bhatkande’s monumental anthology (1965/1910-32) the examples of rāga Alhaiya bilawal provide a basis for contriving stimuli that obey and disobey Alhaiya bilawal’s rules. A study tested the hypothesis that after ten minutes of exposure to Alhaiya bilawal, subjects would correctly distinguish between instances of the rāga and examples that diverged. Fifteen subjects heard 20 two-tala-cycle passages from Bhatkande’s model melodies for rāga Alhaiya bilawal. They then heard 20 passages of the same length and musical format: five from the first session, five from Bhatkande’s anthology that had not previously been heard, and ten that diverged. Subjects indicated which of the ten divergent-plus-lure pairs sounded like the passages heard in the first session. A majority of the non-Indian/nonmusician subjects correctly identified the ten divergent stimuli in the second session. Subjects correctly identified stimuli that diverged from the rules of virtually all rāgas and families of rāgas more often than those that diverged from the more constrained rules of Alhaiya bilawal. A pair of stimulus pairs that resulted in correct identifications by relatively few subjects involved transformations of Clough and Douthett’s usual diatonic collection into anhemitonic pentatonic (1991: cf. also Rahn 1999). An implicit learning design holds promise for corroborating and clarifying cross culturally various musical systems’ claims to universal validity.

Rhythm perception and music cognition: a brief survey

Revista VÓRTEX, 2017

How does our mind process musical rhythm? Based on recent researches and theories on music cognition, the present paper seeks to clarify this question by discussing some issues related to rhythm perception, such as the mental processes involved in the recognition, coding, and retrieval of rhythm and the influence of enculturation and formal musical training on these processes.

Effects of perceptual experience on children's and adults’ perception of unfamiliar rhythms

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2012

Rhythm and meter are fundamental components of music that are universal yet also culture specific. Although simple, isochronous meters are preferred and more readily discriminated than highly complex, nonisochronous meters, moderately complex nonisochronous meters do not pose a problem for listeners who are exposed to them from a young age. The present work uses a behavioral task to examine the ease with which listeners of various ages acquire knowledge of unfamiliar metrical structures from passive exposure. We examined perception of familiar (Western) rhythms with an isochronous meter and unfamiliar (Balkan) rhythms with a nonisochronous meter. We compared discrimination by American children (5 to 11 years) and adults before and after a 2-week period of athome listening to nonisochronous meter music from Bulgaria. During the first session, listeners of all ages exhibited superior discrimination of isochronous than in nonisochronous melodies. Across sessions, this asymmetry declined for young children but not for older children and adults.

Real-time probing of modulations in South Indian classical (Carnātic) music by Indian and Western musicians

We used Toiviainen and Krumhansl's (2003) concurrent probe-tone technique to track Indian and Western musicians' tonal-hierarchy profiles through modulations in Carnātic (South Indian classical) music. Changes of mode (rāgam) are particularly interesting in Carnātic music because of the large number of modes (more than 300) in its tonal system. We first had musicians generate profiles to establish a baseline for each of four rāgams in isolation. Then we obtained dynamic profiles of two modulating excerpts, each of which incorporated two of the four baseline rāgams. The two excerpts used the two techniques of modulation in Carnātic music: grahabēdham (analogous to a Western shift from C major to A minor), and rāgamālikā (analogous to a shift from C major to C minor). We assessed listeners' tracking of the modulations by plotting the correlations of their response profiles with the baseline profiles. In general, the correlation to the original rāgam declined and the correlation to the new rāgam increased with the modulation, and then followed the reverse pattern when the original rāgam returned. Westerners' responses matched those of the Indians on rāgams with structures similar to Western scales, but differed when rāgams were less familiar, and surprisingly, they registered the shifts more strongly than Indian musicians. These findings converged with previous research in identifying three types of cues: 1) culture-specific cues—schematic and veridical knowledge—employed by Indians, 2) tone-distribution cues—duration and frequency of note occurrence—employed by both Indians and Westerners, and 3) transference of schematic knowledge of Western music by Western participants.

A cross-cultural comparison study of the production of simple rhythmic patterns

Psychology of Music, 2004

It has been argued that Japanese and western musicians give different impressions to the listener when performing western music (Saito, 1999; Shibata, 1987). However, these claims are mostly based on subjective impressions and very few studies provide corroborative empirical evidence. The aim of the present study is to compare Japanese and western musicians with regard to timing. Japanese and Dutch percussionists performed nine kinds of rhythmic patterns consisting of two intervals, under two conditions, in three tempi. There seemed to be a tendency for the Japanese participants to perform 4:1, 5:1, 1:4 and 1:5 patterns with a smaller duration ratio than instructed, although performance of patterns with ratios closer to 1 were similar between the participant groups.