Differences in the Association between Segment and Language: Early Bilinguals Pattern with Monolinguals and Are Less Accurate than Late Bilinguals (original) (raw)
Abstract
Early bilinguals often show as much sensitivity to L2-specific contrasts as monolingual speakers of the L2, but most work on cross-language speech perception has focused on isolated segments, and typically only on neighboring vowels or stop contrasts. In tasks that include sounds in context, listeners' success is more variable, so segment discrimination in isolation may not adequately represent the phonetic detail in stored representations. The current study explores the relationship between language experience and sensitivity to segmental cues in context by comparing the categorization patterns of monolingual English listeners and early and late Spanish–English bilinguals. Participants categorized nonce words containing different classes of English-and Spanish-specific sounds as being more English-like or more Spanish-like; target segments included phonemic cues, cues for which there is no analogous sound in the other language, or phonetic cues, cues for which English and Spanish share the category but for which each language varies in its phonetic implementation. Listeners' language categorization accuracy and reaction times were analyzed. Our results reveal a largely uniform categorization pattern across listener groups: Spanish cues were categorized more accurately than English cues, and phonemic cues were easier for listeners to categorize than phonetic cues. There were no differences in the sensitivity of monolinguals and early bilinguals to language-specific cues, suggesting that the early bilinguals' exposure to Spanish did not fundamentally change their representations of English phonology. However, neither did the early bilinguals show more sensitivity than the monolinguals to Spanish sounds. The late bilinguals however, were significantly more accurate than either of the other groups. These findings indicate that listeners with varying exposure to English and Spanish are able to use language-specific cues in a nonce-word language categorization task. Differences in how, and not only when, a language was acquired may influence listener sensitivity to more difficult cues, and the advantage for phonemic cues may reflect the greater salience of categories unique to each language. Implications for foreign-accent categorization and cross-language speech perception are discussed, and future directions are outlined to better understand how salience varies across language-specific phonemic and phonetic cues.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
References (60)
- Allen, J. S., and Miller, J. L. (2004). Listener sensitivity to individual talker di erences in voice-onset-time. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115, 3171-3183.
- Amengual, M. (2014). The perception and production of language-specific mid- vowel contrasts: shifting the to the bilingual individual in early language input conditions. Int. J. Biling. 20, 133-152. doi: 10.1177/1367006914544988
- Amengual, M. (2015). The perception of language-specific phonetic categories does not guarantee accurate phonological representations in the lexicon of early bilinguals. Appl. Psycholinguist. 1-31. doi: 10.1017/S0142716415000557
- Anthony, J. L., and Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14, 255-259. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005. 00376.x
- Best, C. T. (1991). The emergence of native-language phonological influences in infants: a perceptual assimilation model. Haskins Lab. Status Rep. Speech Res. SR-107/108, 1-30.
- Bialystok, E. (2001). Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annu. Rev. Appl. Linguist. 21, 169-181.
- Boersma, P., and Weenink, D. (2012). Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer [Computer Program]. Version 5.3.32. Available at: http://www.praat.org/ Bradlow, A. (1995). A comparative acoustic study of English and Spanish vowels. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 97, 1916-1924. doi: 10.1121/1.412064
- Bradlow, A., Clopper, C., Smiljanic, R., and Walter, M. A. (2010). A perceptual phonetic similarity space for languages: evidence from five native language listener groups. Speech Commun. 52, 930-942. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2010.06.003
- Bruck, M., and Genesee, F. (1995). Phonological awareness in young second language learners. J. Child Lang. 22, 307-324. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900
- Carlson, M. T., Goldrick, M., Blasingame, M., and Fink, A. (2015). Navigating conflicting phonotactic constraints in bilingual speech perception. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1-16. doi: 10.1017/S1366728915000334
- Chan, C. L. (2014). NUsub-db: Northwestern University Subject Database [Web Application].
- Clark, E. V. (2003). First Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Clopper, C. G., and Pisoni, D. B. (2004). Homebodies and army brats: some e ects of early linguistic experience and residential history on dialect categorization. Lang. Var. Change 16, 31-48. doi: 10.1017/S0954394504161036
- Clopper, C. G., and Pisoni, D. B. (2007). Free classification of regional dialects of American English. J. Phon. 35, 421-438. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2006.06.001
- Clopper, C. G., Pisoni, D. B., and de Jong, K. (2005). Acoustic characteristics of the vowel systems of six regional varieties of American English. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 118, 1661-1676. doi: 10.1121/1.2000774
- Costa, A., and Caramazza, A. (1999). Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish-English and English-Spanish bilinguals. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2, 231-244. doi: 10.1017/S1366728999000334
- Cutler, A., Weber, A., Smits, R., and Cooper, N. (2004). Patterns of English phoneme confusions by native and non-native listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 3668-3678. doi: 10.1121/1.1810292
- D ąbrowska, E., and Street, J. A. (2006). Individual di erences in language attainment: comprehension of passive sentences by native and non-native English speakers. Lang. Sci. 28, 604-615. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2005.11.014
- Diehl, R. L., and Walsh, M. A. (1989). An auditory basis for the stimulus-length e ect in the perception of stops and glides. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 2154-2164. doi: 10.1121/1.397864
- Dodd, B., Holm, A., Hua, Z., and Crosbie, S. (2003). Phonological development: a normative study of British English-speaking children. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 17, 617-643. doi: 10.1080/0269920031000111348
- Escudero, P. (2006). "The phonological and phonetic development of new vowel contrasts in Spanish learners of English, " in English With a Latin Beat: Studies in Portuguese/Spanish-English Interphonology, Studies in Bilingualism, Vol. 31, eds B. O. Baptista and M. A. Watkins (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 149-161.
- Flege, J. E. (1984). The detection of French accent by American listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 76, 692-707. doi: 10.1121/1.391256
- Flege, J. E. (1987). The production of "new" and "similar" phones in a foreign language: evidence for the e ect of equivalence classification. J. Phon. 15, 47-55.
- Flege, J. E. (1991). Age of learning a ects the authenticity of voice onset time (VOT) in stop consonants produced in a second language. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 89, 395-411. doi: 10.1121/1.400473
- Flege, J. E. (1995). "Second language speech learning: theory, findings, and problems, " in Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross- Language Research, ed. W. Strange (Timonium, MD: York Press), 233-277.
- Flege, J. E. (2007). "Language contact in bilingualism: phonetic system interactions, " in Laboratory Phonology 9, eds J. Cole and J. Hualde (Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter), 353-382.
- Flege, J. E., Bohn, O.-S., and Jang, S. (1997a). E ects of experience on non-native speakers' production and perception of English vowels. J. Phon. 25, 437-470. doi: 10.1121/1.3466879
- Flege, J. E., Frieda, E. M., and Nozawa, T. (1997b). Amount of native-language (L1) use a ects the pronunciation of an L2. J. Phon. 25, 169-186. doi: 10.1006/jpho.1996.0040
- Flege, J. E., and MacKay, I. R. A. (2004). Perceiving vowels in a second language. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 26, 1-34. doi: 10.1159/000235659
- Flege, J. E., MacKay, I. R. A., and Meador, D. (1999a). Native Italian speakers' perception and production of English vowels. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106, 2973- 2987. doi: 10.1121/1.428116
- Flege, J. E., and Munro, M. J. (1994). The word unit in second language speech production and perception. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 16, 381-411. doi: 10.1017/S0272263100013437
- Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., and Fox, R. A. (1994). Auditory and categorical e ects on cross-language vowel perception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95, 3623-3641. doi: 10.1121/1.409931
- Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., and Liu, S. (1999b). Age constraints on second- language acquisition. J. Mem. Lang. 41, 78-104. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2638
- Gelman, A., Lee, D., and Guo, J. (2015). A probabilistic programming language for bayesian inference and optimization. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 40, 530-543. doi: 10.3102/1076998615606113
- Gertken, L. M., Amengual, M., and Birdsong, D. (2014). "Assessing language dominance with the Bilingual Language Profile, " in Measuring L2 proficiency: Perspectives from SLA, eds P. Leclercq, A. Edmonds, and H. Hilton (Bristol: Multilingual Matters), 208-225.
- Harnsberger, J. D., Shrivastav, R., Brown, W. S. Jr., Rothman, H., and Hollien, H. (1997). Speaking rate and fundamental frequency as speech cues to perceived age. J. Voice 22, 58-69. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.07.004
- Hualde, J. I. (2005). The of Spanish. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
- Johnson, K. (1997). "Speech perception without speaker normalization, " in Talker Variability in Speech Processing, eds K. Johnson and J. W. Mullennix (New York, NY: Academic Press), 145-166.
- Klatt, D. H., and Klatt, L. C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87, 820-857. doi: 10.1121/1.398894
- Kondaurova, M. V., and Francis, A. L. (2008). The relationship between native allophonic experience with vowel duration and perception of the English tense/lax vowel contrast by Spanish and Russian listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 124, 3959-3971. doi: 10.1121/1.2999341
- Kraljic, T., and Samuel, A. G. (2006). Generalization in perceptual learning for speech. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 262-268. doi: 10.3758/BF03193841
- Kraljic, T., and Samuel, A. G. (2007). Perceptual adjustments to multiple speakers. J. Mem. Lang. 56, 1-15. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0856-y
- Mack, M. (1989). Consonant and vowel perception and production: early English- French bilinguals and English monolinguals. Percept. Psychophys. 46, 187-200. doi: 10.3758/BF03204982
- Mendez, A. (1982). Production of American English and Spanish vowels. Lang. Speech 25, 191-197. doi: 10.1177/002383098202500206
- Munro, M. J., and Derwing, T. M. (2001). Modeling perceptions of the accentedness and comprehensibility of L2 Speech: the role of speaking rate. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 23, 451-468.
- Nygaard, L. C., and Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Talker-specific learning in speech perception. Percept. Psychphys. 60, 355-376. doi: 10.3758/BF032 06860
- Pennington, B. F., van Orden, G. C., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., and Haith, M. M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics. Child Dev. 61, 1753-1778. doi: 10.2307/1130836
- Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2002). "Word-specific phonetics, " in Laboratory Phonology, Vol. 7, eds C. Gussenhoven and N. Warner (Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter), 101-139.
- Pisoni, D. B. (1977). Identification and discrimination of the relative voice onset time of two component tones: implication for voicing perception in stops. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 61, 1352-1361. doi: 10.1121/1.38 1409 Psychology Software Tools (2010). Psychology Software Tools, Inc. E-Prime 2.0. Available at: http://www.pstnet.com
- Recasens, D. (2004). Darkness in [l] as a scalar phonetic property: implications for phonology and articulatory control. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 18, 593-603. doi: 10.1080/02699200410001703556
- Recasens, D. (2012). A cross-language acoustic study of initial and final allophones of [l]. Speech Commun. 54, 368-383. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2011. 10.001
- RStudio Team (2015). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, Inc. Boston, MA: RStudio Team. Available at: http://www.rstudio.com/ Sebastián-Gallés, N., Echeverría, S., and Bosch, L. (2005). The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. J. Mem. Lang. 52, 240-255. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.11.001
- Sebastián-Gallés, N., and Soto-Faraco, S. (1999). Online processing of native and non-native phonemic contrasts in early bilinguals. Cognition 72, 111-123. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00024-4
- Shook, A., and Marian, V. (2013). The Bilingual language interaction network for comprehension of speech. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 16, 304-324. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000466
- Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., and van der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit (with discussion). J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 64, 583-639. doi: 10.1111/1467-9868.00353
- Strand, E. A. (1999). Uncovering the role of gender stereotypes in speech perception. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 18, 86-99. doi: 10.1177/0261927X99018001006
- Strand, E. A., and Johnson, K. (1996). "Gradient and visual speaker normalization in the perception of fricatives, " in Natural Language Processing and Speech Technology: Results of the 3rd KONVENS Conference, Bielefeld, ed. D. Gibbon (Berlin: Mouton), 14-26.
- Tracy, E. C., Bainter, S. A., and Satariano, N. P. (2015). Judgments of self- identified gay and heterosexual male speakers: which phonemes are the most salient in determining sexual orientation? J. Phon. 52, 13-25. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2015.04.001
- Vieru, B., Boula de Mareüil, P., and Adda-Decker, M. (2011). Characterisation and identification of non-native French accents. Speech Commun. 53, 292-310. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2010.10.002
- Witteman, M. J., Weber, A., and McQueen, J. M. (2013). Foreign accent strength and listener familiarity with an accent codetermine speed of perceptual adaptation. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 75, 537-556. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012- 0404-y