The capacity of humans to identify components in complex odor-taste mixtures. Chem. Senses 31:539–545 (original) (raw)

The analysis of odor mixtures by humans: evidence for a configurational process

Physiology & Behavior, 2001

Humans have a limited capacity to analyze odor mixtures with three to four being the maximum [Physiol Behav 46 (1989) 809.]. This study investigates the large loss of information about odor identity that occurs in mixtures and aims to determine the information on which identification and failure to identify is based. In Experiment 1, 14 subjects used a selective attention procedure to identify odorants in stimuli consisting of one to four components. As expected, substantial difficulties were encountered in identifying more than two odorants, and chance level scores were obtained for the group for each of the odorants in the quaternary mixture. In Experiment 2, 21 subjects used a profiling procedure consisting of 146 descriptors to describe the odor qualities perceived in the same stimuli used in Experiment 1. The results indicated that for some odorants, loss of a major characteristic quality occurred even in binary mixtures, but that many of the features of some odorants remained in the quaternary mixture. Comparison of the data from the two experiments indicated that identification of most of the prominent qualities of an odorant was not necessarily sufficient for identification of the odorant in a mixture. In contrast, the loss of some prominent features did not always result in non-identification. A configurational hypothesis of olfaction, analogous to that for facial and object recognition, is proposed to account for the data and the processes underlying odor identification in mixtures.

Perceptual Processing Strategy and Exposure Influence the Perception of Odor Mixtures

Chemical Senses, 2007

In flavor perception, both experience with the components of odor/taste mixtures and the cognitive strategy used to examine the interactions between the components influence the overall mixture perception. However, the effect of these factors on odor mixtures perception has never been studied. The present study aimed at evaluating whether 1) previous exposure to the odorants included in a mixture or 2) the synthetic or analytic strategy engaged during odorants mixture evaluation determines odor representation. Blending mixtures, in which subjects perceived a unique quality distinct from those of components, were chosen in order to induce a priori synthetic perception. In the first part, we checked whether the chosen mixtures presented blending properties for our subjects. In the second part, 3 groups of participants were either exposed to the odorants contributing to blending mixtures with a ''pineapple'' or a ''red cordial'' odor or nonexposed. In a following task, half of each group was assigned to a synthetic or an analytical task. The synthetic task consisted of rating how typical (i.e., representative) of the target odor name (pineapple or red cordial) were the mixtures and each of their components. The analytical task consisted of evaluating these stimuli on several scales labeled with the target odor name and odor descriptors of the components. Previous exposure to mixture components was found to decrease mixture typicality but only for the pineapple blending mixture. Likewise, subjects engaged in an analytical task rated both blending mixtures as less typical than did subjects engaged in a synthetic task. This study supports a conclusion that odor mixtures can be perceived either analytically or synthetically according to the cognitive strategy engaged.

The influence of chemical complexity on the perception of multicomponent odor mixtures

Perception & Psychophysics, 1998

The present study investigates the hypothesis that complex object odors (odors that emanate from flowers, foods, sewage, etc.) that consist of dozens of odorants are processed and encoded as discrete entities, as if each was a single chemical odor. To test this hypothesis, the capacity of trained subjects to discriminate and identify the components of stimuli consisting of one to eight object odors was determined. The results indicated that subjects could only identify up to four object odors in a mixture, which is similar to earlier findings with mixtures that contained only single chemical odors. The limited capacity was also reflected in the number of odors selected, regardless of whether the choices were correct or incorrect, in confidence ratings, and in decision times. The identification of a limited number of object odors in every mixture that was presented suggests that both associative (synthetic) and dissociative (analytic) processes are involved in the perceptual analysis of odor mixtures.

Taste-taste, odor-odor, and taste-odor mixtures: Greater suppression within than between modalities

Perception & Psychophysics, 1983

Subjects judged the intensities of the component qualities of two taste stimuli [sucrose (S) and NaCI (N)), two odor stimuli [citral (C) and anethole (A)), and the mixtures of these stimuli [taste-taste (SN), odor-odor (CA), and taste-odor (SC, NC, SA, and NA)). The withinmodality mixtures, taste-taste and odor-odor, produced greater suppression of perceived intensity than did the between-modality mixtures, taste-odor. However, taste-odor mixtures also produced a significant amount of suppression of perceived intensity when compared with the individual taste and odor stimuli. The data are discussed with respect to peripheral and central mechanisms in mixture suppression. 183

Odor/taste integration and the perception of flavor

Experimental Brain Research, 2005

Perceptions of the flavors of foods or beverages reflect information derived from multiple sensory afferents, including gustatory, olfactory, and somatosensory fibers. Although flavor perception therefore arises from the central integration of multiple sensory inputs, it is possible to distinguish the different modalities contributing to flavor, especially when attention is drawn to particular sensory characteristics. Nevertheless, our experiences of the flavor of a food or beverage are also simultaneously of an overall unitary perception. Research aimed at understanding the mechanisms behind this integrated flavor perception is, for the most part, relatively recent. However, psychophysical, neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies on cross-modal sensory interactions involved in flavor perception have started to provide an understanding of the integrated activity of sensory systems that generate such unitary perceptions, and hence the mechanisms by which these signals are functionally united when anatomically separated . Here we review this recent research on odor/taste integration, and propose a model of flavor processing that depends on prior experience with the particular combination of sensory inputs, temporal and spatial concurrence, and attentional allocation. We propose that flavor perception depends upon neural processes occurring in chemosensory regions of the brain, including the anterior insula, frontal operculum, orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex, as well as upon the interaction of this chemosensory flavor network with other heteromodal regions including the posterior parietal cortex and possibly the ventral lateral prefrontal cortex.

The Acquisition of Taste Properties by Odors

Certain odors are routinely described as smelling sweet, This phenomenon may result from the co-occurrence of such odors and tastes outside the laboratory. Experiment 1 tested this possibility by pairing a selected odor with sucrose and another with citric acid in a masked design, using 24-h spaced sessions, preceded and followed by ratings of the odors' taste attributes when sniffed and when rated with tastes in solution. Following conditioning, the odor paired with sucrose smelled sweeter and with citric acid, sourer. In Experiment 2, contingency awareness was examined using a recognition measure, in an otherwise similar design. Again, odors smelled sweeter and sourer postconditioning. Contingency aware and unaware subjects did not differ in performance. Experiment 3 examined an exposure account of these changes, using a similar paradigm to Experiment 1, but with no exposures to sucrose or citric acid. No changes in odor taste attributes were observed. Overall, these findings demonstrate that associative learning, irrespective of awareness, has an important role in tile acquisition of odor-taste qualities.

Selective attention and the perceptual analysis of odor mixtures

Physiology & behavior, 1992

Two psychophysical methods were used to investigate the capacity of humans to identify the constituents of odor mixtures consisting of up to six components. With one method subjects were required to identify all the components present in each stimulus; with the other, a selective attention procedure was used where subjects had to identify only one component at each trial. Little difference was found between the levels of identification obtained with both methods, reinforcing the finding that humans have great difficulty in identifying more than three components in an odor mixture and indicating that it is unlikely that olfactory adaptation influenced the identification process.

“Taste-smell confusions” and the duality of the olfactory sense

Perception & Psychophysics, 1982

In this paper, I argue that olfaction is the only dual sensory modality, in that it senses both objects in the external world and objects in the body (mouth). I suggest that the same olfactory stimulation may be perceived and evaluated in two qualitatively different ways, depending on whether it is referred to the mouth or the external world. I begin with a discussion of disparities in the affective value of odors and tastes of the same substance. On eating a piece of Limburger cheese, a friend once commented to me: "I really love the taste, if I can only get it by my nose." The absurdity of this statement hit me head-on. Surely, the odor of the cheese is the primary cause of its repulsiveness. Yet, this same odor, which also constitutes the most distinctive aspect of its "taste," becomes pleasant when it is sensed in conjunction with ingestion. This affect reversal is no rarity. I asked 42 undergraduates whether they disliked the smell, but liked the taste, of any foods. Specific categories were suggested (strong cheese, fish, eggs, vegetables, fruits, meat), although subjects were free to indicate items outside of these classes. More than half of the subjects (22) indicated such a response to strong cheese. The 42 subjects generated 72 instances, with fish (15) and eggs (11) following strong cheese in frequency. The opposite affect reversal, liking the smell and disliking the taste, is yet more common. Black, unsweetened coffee is a common example. However, this type of reversal presents no interesting psychological problem. The taste (in the narrow sense) properties causing the dislike are never sensed in the external object. While the odor is shared by an object-at-a-distance and an object-in-the-mouth, the taste appears only with the latter. So, black coffee is disliked in the mouth because a bitter taste is added to a pleasant odor. It is well known that affective responses are context dependent. The same foul odor can be pleasant Some of the research described in this paper was supported by National Science Foundation Grant BNS 76 80108. I thank Fritzsche-D&O for providing flavor extracts that were components of some of the exotic fruit juice stimuli; Susan Fahrbach, Christopher Earl, Elise Hochfeld, and Michael Mark for participation in the design and data collection in the study on identification of the tastes associated with learned smells; and

Learning Influences the Perception of Odor Mixtures

In this study, we investigated the influence of semantic and perceptual learning on the perception of odor blending mixtures, i.e., mixtures eliciting a different odor quality as compared with their components. Following a between-groups design, we set out to examine whether being pre-exposed to target mixture odor labels (semantic learning) and to the mixture's odor components (perceptual olfactory learning) would modify the perception of odor mixtures quality. In a first experimental condition, a group (control) of 28 subjects evaluated the odor quality of mixtures and then their components. In this condition, subjects were not exposed to the individual odor components (no perceptual learning) and were only weakly exposed to the target mixture odor labels (weak semantic learning) when asked to evaluate the quality of the mixtures. In a second experimental condition, a group (learning) of 29 other subjects performed the same tasks but evaluated first the components and then the mixtures. In this condition, subjects have experienced both perceptual and strong semantic learning when asked to evaluate the quality of the mixtures. Within each condition, odor quality was evaluated following two verbal tasks: a free description task and a choice between several odor attributes. The comparison of the data obtained in both experimental conditions revealed that semantic and perceptual learning could influence odor mixture quality and suggested a differential influence of both learning types according to mixture complexity.

Preexposure to the stimulus elements, but not training to detect them, retards human odour-taste learning

Behavioural Processes, 2003

Odours are judged to smell sweeter following simultaneous oral pairings with the tastant sucrose and sourer after parings with the tastant citric acid. This effect may result from human participants perceiving and encoding a unitary odour-taste percept. This study examined two factors thought likely to disrupt such encoding; (a) preexposure to the mixture elements and (b) training to spot the elements of taste-odour mixtures. Half of the participants were trained to identify tastes and smells and half received no training. All participants were preexposed to two odours (A, B) and two tastes (X, Y), followed by pairings of these stimuli (AX, BY) and then by pairings between two non-preexposed odours and the same tastes (CX, DY). This process was then repeated on a second session. Odour-taste learning was retarded following preexposure, but was unaffected by training. These findings suggest; (1) that odour-taste mixtures may be cognitively impenetrable and (2) that preexposure leads to encoding of A and B, which are then resistant to interference when further pairings are presented (i.e. AX, BY).