Stereopsis Research Papers - Academia.edu (original) (raw)
PURPOSE. Human vision has a puzzling stereoscopic anisotropy: horizontal depth corrugations are easier to detect than vertical depth corrugations. To date, little is known about the function or the underlying mechanism responsible for... more
PURPOSE. Human vision has a puzzling stereoscopic anisotropy: horizontal depth corrugations are easier to detect than vertical depth corrugations. To date, little is known about the function or the underlying mechanism responsible for this anisotropy. Here, we aim to find out whether this anisotropy is independent of age. To answer this, we compare detection thresholds for horizontal and vertical depth corrugations as a function of age. METHODS. The depth corrugations were defined solely by the horizontal disparity of random dot patterns. The disparities depicted a horizontal or vertical sinusoidal depth corrugation of spatial frequency 0.1 cyc/deg. Detection thresholds were obtained using Bayesian adaptive staircases from a total of 159 subjects aged from 3 to 73 years. For each participant we computed the anisotropy index, defined as the log 10-ratio of the detection threshold for vertical corrugations divided by that for horizontal. RESULTS. Anisotropy index was highly variable between individuals but was positive in 87% of the participants. There was a significant correlation between anisotropy index and log-age (r ¼ 0.21, P ¼ 0.008) mainly driven by a significant difference between children and adults. In 67 children aged 3 to 13 years, the mean anisotropy index was 0.34 6 0.38 (mean 6 SD, meaning that vertical thresholds were on average 2.2 times the horizontal ones), compared with 0.59 6 0.55 in 84 adults aged 18 to 73 years (vertical 3.9 times horizontal). This was mainly driven by a decline in the sensitivity to vertical corrugations. Children had poorer stereoacuity than adults, but had similar sensitivity to adults for horizontal corrugations and were actually more sensitive than adults to vertical corrugations. CONCLUSIONS. The fact that adults show stronger stereo anisotropy than children raises the possibility that visual experience plays a critical role in developing and strengthening the stereo anisotropy. B inocular stereopsis is the process that allows us to recover the relative depth of objects from the binocular disparities between the images of the world projected into both eyes. Experiments using random dot stereograms 1 to generate depth corrugations defined by horizontal disparities have shown the existence of a strong anisotropy in depth perception: at spatial frequencies lower than approximately 0.4 cyc/deg, horizontally oriented sinusoidal depth corrugations are much easier to detect than vertical corrugations. 2–6 This anisotropy is similar to that found in sensitivity to disparity-defined slanted surfaces, where the sensitivity is greater for surfaces that rotated around the horizontal axis than for surfaces that rotated around the vertical axis. 7–11 Despite the strong evidence for this stereoscopic anisotropy, little is known about its function or the underlying mechanisms involved. Recently, we proposed a novel mechanistic explanation for stereoscopic anisotropy: that humans have only a single spatial-frequency channel for vertically oriented disparity corrugations, tuned to frequencies above 0.4 cyc/deg, but have at least two channels for horizontal corrugations, with a channel tuned to frequencies below 0.4 cyc/deg responsible for the greater sensitivity observed for horizontal corrugations at low frequencies. 5 However, direct tests of this speculation using masking experiments 4 have found multiple disparity channels for both orientations. Witz and Hess, 12 using spatially band-pass noise rather than random dots, also concluded that vertical and horizontal stereo corrugations are detected by multiple disparity channels. 13 Thus, the single versus multiple disparity channel hypothesis for explaining the stereoscopic anisotropy cannot be sustained. Another possible explanation of stereoscopic anisotropy relates to the anisotropy of summation fields. Tyler and Kontsevich 14 found that summation fields extended only in the horizontal orientation and increased in size as the spatial frequency of the Gabor depth ripples was reduced (aspect ratio 4:1 cycles). They argue that the presence of high contrast vertical contours support stereopsis in vertical objects like branches, whereas for horizontal contours, only the surface texture supports stereoscopic depth. Thus, they suggest that horizontally elongated summation fields are needed to compensate the differences in disparity and luminance information present in natural images. According to this compensation mechanism, if we measure disparity thresholds for vertical and horizontal sinusoidal corrugations of low spatial frequency and iovs.arvojournals.org j