A speech planning network for interactive language use (original) (raw)
Data availability
The data used in these analyses are not publicly available owing to concerns regarding patient privacy; however, the corresponding author will provide deidentified primary data upon request.
Code availability
The corresponding author will provide the MATLAB code used in this study for analysis of ECoG and behavioural data upon request.
References
- Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A. & Jefferson, G. A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 50, 696–735 (1974).
Article Google Scholar - Levinson, S. C. & Torreira, F. Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Front. Psychol. 6, 731 (2015).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Stivers, T. et al. Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 10587–10592 (2009).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Schiffman, H. R. Sensation and Perception: An Integrated Approach (Wiley, 2001).
- Flinker, A. et al. Redefining the role of Broca’s area in speech. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 2871–2875 (2015).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Basilakos, A., Smith, K. G., Fillmore, P., Fridriksson, J. & Fedorenko, E. Functional characterization of the human speech articulation network. Cereb. Cortex 28, 1816–1830 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Mirman, D., Kraft, A. E., Harvey, D. Y., Brecher, A. R. & Schwartz, M. F. Mapping articulatory and grammatical subcomponents of fluency deficits in post-stroke aphasia. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 19, 1286–1298 (2019).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Guenther, F. H. Neural Control of Speech (MIT, 2016).
- Sahin, N. T., Pinker, S., Cash, S. S., Schomer, D. & Halgren, E. Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within Broca’s area. Science 326, 445–449 (2009).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Broca, P. Remarques sur le siege de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d’une observation d’aphémie (perte de la parole). Bull. Mem. Soc. Anat. Paris 36, 330–356 (1861).
Google Scholar - Chang, E. F. et al. Pure apraxia of speech after resection based in the posterior middle frontal gyrus. Neurosurgery 87, E383–E389 (2020).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Brass, M. & von Cramon, D. Y. The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cereb. Cortex 12, 908–914 (2002).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Sierpowska, J. et al. Involvement of the middle frontal gyrus in language switching as revealed by electrical stimulation mapping and functional magnetic resonance imaging in bilingual brain tumor patients. Cortex 99, 78–92 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Levinson, S. C. Turn-taking in human communication-origins and implications for language processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 6–14 (2016).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Indefrey, P. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: a critical update. Front. Psychol. 2, 255 (2011).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Schuhmann, T., Schiller, N. O., Goebel, R. & Sack, A. T. The temporal characteristics of functional activation in Broca’s area during overt picture naming. Cortex 45, 1111–1116 (2009).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Ferpozzi, V. et al. Broca’s area as a pre-articulatory phonetic encoder: gating the motor program. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12, 64 (2018).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Alario, F. X., Chainay, H., Lehericy, S. & Cohen, L. The role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in word production. Brain Res. 1076, 129–143 (2006).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Ramanarayanan, V., Goldstein, L., Byrd, D. & Narayanan, S. S. An investigation of articulatory setting using real-time magnetic resonance imaging. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134, 510–519 (2013).
Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Bogels, S., Magyari, L. & Levinson, S. C. Neural signatures of response planning occur midway through an incoming question in conversation. Sci Rep. 5, 12881 (2015).
Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Ferreira, F. & Swets, B. How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums. J. Mem. Lang. 46, 57–84 (2002).
Article Google Scholar - Wagner, V., Jescheniak, J. D. & Schriefers, H. On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 36, 423–440 (2010).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Dubey, A. & Ray, S. Cortical electrocorticogram (ECoG) is a local signal. J. Neurosci. 39, 4299–4311 (2019).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Cheung, C., Hamiton, L. S., Johnson, K. & Chang, E. F. The auditory representation of speech sounds in human motor cortex. eLife 5, e12577 (2016).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Glanz Iljina, O. et al. Real-life speech production and perception have a shared premotor-cortical substrate. Sci. Rep. 8, 8898 (2018).
Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F. Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 269–298 (2010).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Ray, S. & Maunsell, J. H. Different origins of gamma rhythm and high-gamma activity in macaque visual cortex. PLoS Biol. 9, e1000610 (2011).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Flinker, A., Chang, E. F., Barbaro, N. M., Berger, M. S. & Knight, R. T. Sub-centimeter language organization in the human temporal lobe. Brain Lang. 117, 103–109 (2011).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Bouchard, K. E., Mesgarani, N., Johnson, K. & Chang, E. F. Functional organization of human sensorimotor cortex for speech articulation. Nature 495, 327–332 (2013).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Cogan, G. B. et al. Sensory-motor transformations for speech occur bilaterally. Nature 507, 94–98 (2014).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Kotz, S. A. et al. Lexicality drives audio-motor transformations in Broca’s area. Brain Lang. 112, 3–11 (2010).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Fadiga, L. & Craighero, L. Hand actions and speech representation in Broca’s area. Cortex 42, 486–490 (2006).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Knudsen, B., Creemers, A. & Meyer, A. S. Forgotten little words: how backchannels and particles may facilitate speech planning in conversation? Front. Psychol. 11, 593671 (2020).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Long, M. A. et al. Functional segregation of cortical regions underlying speech timing and articulation. Neuron 89, 1187–1193 (2016).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Tate, M. C., Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S., Tate, J. E. & Duffau, H. Probabilistic map of critical functional regions of the human cerebral cortex: Broca’s area revisited. Brain 137, 2773–2782 (2014).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Long, M. A. & Fee, M. S. Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature 456, 189–194 (2008).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Okobi, D. E., Jr, Banerjee, A., Matheson, A. M. M., Phelps, S. M. & Long, M. A. Motor cortical control of vocal interaction in neotropical singing mice. Science 363, 983–988 (2019).
Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Tremblay, P. & Dick, A. S. Broca and Wernicke are dead, or moving past the classic model of language neurobiology. Brain Lang. 162, 60–71 (2016).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Hosman, T. et al. Auditory cues reveal intended movement information in middle frontal gyrus neuronal ensemble activity of a person with tetraplegia. Sci Rep. 11, 98 (2021).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Catani, M. et al. Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain. Cortex 48, 273–291 (2012).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Glasser, M. F. et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536, 171–178 (2016).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Mathis, A. et al. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1281–1289 (2018).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Deger, K. & Ziegler, W. Speech motor programming in apraxia of speech. J. Phon. 30, 321–335 (2002).
Article Google Scholar - Jackson, E. S. et al. A fNIRS investigation of speech planning and execution in adults who stutter. Neuroscience 406, 73–85 (2019).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Bogels, S., Casillas, M. & Levinson, S. C. Planning versus comprehension in turn-taking: fast responders show reduced anticipatory processing of the question. Neuropsychologia 109, 295–310 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Dale, A. M., Fischl, B. & Sereno, M. I. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194 (1999).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar - Fischl, B. et al. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14, 11–22 (2004).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Klein, A. & Tourville, J. 101 labeled brain images and a consistent human cortical labeling protocol. Front. Neurosci. 6, 171 (2012).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Desikan, R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980 (2006).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Avants, B. B. et al. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54, 2033–2044 (2011).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Tyszka, J. M. & Pauli, W. M. In vivo delineation of subdivisions of the human amygdaloid complex in a high-resolution group template. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3979–3998 (2016).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Kovach, C. K. & Gander, P. E. The demodulated band transform. J. Neurosci. Methods 261, 135–154 (2016).
Article PubMed Google Scholar - Liu, Y., Coon, W. G., Pesters, A., de, B. P. & Schalk, G. The effects of spatial filtering and artifacts on electrocorticographic signals. J. Neural Eng. 12, 056008 (2015).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar - Friston, K. J. et al. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 189–210 (1995).
Article Google Scholar - Qian, T., Wu, W., Zhou, W., Gao, S. & Hong, B. in Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society 2347–2350.
- Tilsen, S. et al. Anticipatory posturing of the vocal tract reveals dissociation of speech movement plans from linguistic units. PLoS ONE 11, e0146813 (2016).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Acknowledgements
We thank A. Flinker, E. Jackson, J. Krivokapić, D. Schneider, N. Tritsch and members of the Long laboratory for comments on earlier versions of this manuscript; A. Ramirez-Cardenas, H. Chen, K. Ibayashi, H. Kawasaki, K. Nourski, H. Oya, A. Rhone and B. Snoad for help with data collection; and F. Guenther and N. Majaj for helpful conversations. This research was supported by R01 DC019354 (M.A.L.), R01 DC015260 (J.D.W.G.) and Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain (M.A.L.).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
- NYU Neuroscience Institute and Department of Otolaryngology, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
Gregg A. Castellucci & Michael A. Long - Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA
Gregg A. Castellucci & Michael A. Long - Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
Christopher K. Kovach, Matthew A. Howard III & Jeremy D. W. Greenlee
Authors
- Gregg A. Castellucci
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Christopher K. Kovach
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Matthew A. Howard III
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Jeremy D. W. Greenlee
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar - Michael A. Long
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar
Contributions
G.A.C. and M.A.L. conceived the study and designed the experiments; G.A.C., C.K.K., J.D.W.G. and M.A.L. conducted the research; G.A.C., C.K.K. and M.A.L. performed data analyses; G.A.C., C.K.K. and M.A.L. created the figures; G.A.C. and M.A.L. wrote the initial draft of the manuscript; G.A.C., C.K.K., M.A.H., J.D.W.G. and M.A.L. edited and reviewed the final manuscript. J.D.W.G. and M.A.L. acquired funding; J.D.W.G., M.A.H. and M.A.L. supervised the project.
Corresponding author
Correspondence toMichael A. Long.
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review information
Nature thanks Gregory Cogan, Uri Hasson and Frederic Theunissen for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data figures and tables
Extended Data Fig. 1 Behaviour during the CI task.
a, Description of subprocesses assumed to occur during the perception, planning, and production windows of the CI task. b, Histograms of reaction times (RT) in early and late CI trials for all participants. c, Median RT values for early and late CI trials for all participants. d, e, Histograms depicting the distribution of average peak-to-trough response amplitudes for all electrodes displaying planning-related responses when aligned to CI onset in early and late trials (d) and different CI question types (e); median values for each distribution are indicated. Observed data (in black) are compared with a null distribution (in grey) consisting of randomly chosen timepoints (Methods). f, Schematics displaying GLM regressor structure for an early (top) and a late (bottom) variant of an example CI task question.
Extended Data Fig. 2 GLM temporal jittering analysis.
a, Full model R values for GLM fits of jittered high gamma activity from participant 436; each line represents data from an individual electrode. b, Example distribution of pooled D values with the fit of two Gaussians overlaid (black). The Gaussian distributions corresponding to well fit (blue) and poorly fit electrodes (red) as well as the 95th percentile of the D distribution for poorly fit electrodes (dashed line) are indicated. D values above the 95th percentile of the pooled distribution were deemed outliers (white bars) and not fitted. c, Table summarizing the number of electrodes rejected by the jittering analysis in each participant. d, Table reporting the anatomical locations of electrodes rejected by the jittering analysis and electrodes displaying significant activity in the CI task. e, Scatterplot depicting the proportion of rejected electrodes within a region as a function of the proportion of responsive electrodes in a region.
Extended Data Fig. 3 Analysis of neural activity in the CI task.
a, Scatterplot depicting the distribution of all simulated task-responsive electrodes from the continuum model in three-dimensional GLM weight space; cluster membership indicated by greyscale colour. b, c, Distribution of simulated electrodes from the continuum model displaying responses in one window (i.e., unmixed) of the CI task (b) or multiple windows (c); response class indicated by colour in b and c and unmixed electrodes denoted by small black points in c. In b, simulated unmixed electrodes located outside the cluster primarily containing electrodes of the same type (i.e., ‘misclustered’) are indicated with an ‘X’. d, e, Histograms depicting the distribution of the proportion of misclustered electrodes responsive during a single task window (i.e., unmixed electrodes) (d), and the proportion of electrodes displaying more than one significant positive weight (i.e., mixed electrodes) (e) across 100,000 iterations of the continuum model simulation. The median of each distribution as well as the values observed in the actual data (dashed line) are indicated. Gold arrows indicate the bin of each distribution containing the measurements corresponding to the example iteration depicted in panels p, r, and t of Fig. 1. f, Table reporting the number of electrodes displaying perception-related responses using either the full model or the reduced GLM lacking a planning regressor. g, h, Scatterplots depicting perception (g) and planning (h) GLM weights in the full model and reduced models lacking a planning regressor or perception regressor, respectively. Significant positive weights are denoted with filled points and nonsignificant or significant negative weights are denoted with unfilled points; the _x_-coordinates of each point are randomly jittered by 25% to better visualize filled versus unfilled status. No planning electrodes displayed significant perception responses in the reduced GLM lacking a planning regressor, and no perception electrodes displayed significant planning responses in the reduced GLM lacking a perception regressor.
Extended Data Fig. 4 Additional analyses of task-related activity changes.
a, Table reporting the number of perception, planning, and production-related electrodes displaying significant positive and negative weights for each GLM regressor. b, Histogram depicting mean high gamma amplitude in the first 500 ms of CI questions for all unmixed perception, planning, and production electrodes. c, d, Canonical cortical surfaces displaying electrodes with significant positive (coloured) or negative (black) GLM weights in the perception (c), production (d), and planning (e) windows of the CI task across all participants. Electrode diameter is scaled to the absolute magnitude of the GLM weight, and electrodes not displaying a significant weight for a given regressor are indicated with small white circles.
Extended Data Fig. 5 Anatomical analysis of responses.
a, Cortical reconstructions for all participants displaying the location of all electrodes; the size of each electrode depicts the actual size of its recording area on the cortical surface. GLM classification is indicated by electrode colour. b, Canonical cortical surfaces showing electrode locations from all participants as standard-sized white circles. c, Number of electrodes sampling each area of the canonical cortical surface (1 cm diameter spatial smoothing) after pooling electrodes from all participants. d, Proportion of electrodes displaying significant production-related responses in the CI task (1-cm-diameter spatial smoothing). e, Canonical cortical surfaces displaying electrodes with significant responses related to speech perception, production, and planning in patients with tumour (top) and patients with epilepsy (bottom) separately; electrode diameter scaled to GLM regressor weight. Electrodes not displaying a significant response for a process are depicted as small white circles.
Extended Data Fig. 6 Additional conversation-related analyses.
a, Table reporting additional turn-taking behavioural measures for each participant. b, Histograms of gap durations (time between experimenter turn offset and participant turn onset) during unconstrained conversation for each participant; bins are centred on 100 ms increments with a width of 100 ms. c, Scree plots for the PCA analysis of high gamma signals in the task (left) and conversation (right) periods of the recordings; data from each participant are represented by thin lines and the average across participants is denoted with a thick black line. The 95% confidence interval of the linear decay phase across participants (Methods) is also indicated. d, The observed number of electrodes whose cluster membership was not stable (i.e., switched clusters) between the task and conversation with a histogram depicting the distribution of electrode cluster switches expected by chance. e, The observed percentage of electrodes in perception, planning, and production clusters (in conversation-derived PC coefficient space) displaying significant perception, planning, and production responses (per the GLM), respectively, with histograms depicting the percentages expected by chance for each cluster type. f, Canonical cortical surfaces displaying the locations of all electrodes in perception, planning, and production clusters across participants (n = 6) in the task (left) and conversation (right). g, Table reporting summary statistics for PC activity (i.e., time-varying PC score) during unconstrained conversation for each participant.
Extended Data Fig. 7 PCA results for individual participants.
a–f, For 6 participants possessing sufficient numbers of electrodes belonging to multiple GLM classes (Methods): scatterplots depicting electrode distributions in PC coefficient space in the task and conversation periods (top row). Bar graphs depicting the PC coefficients for all electrodes in perception, planning, or production clusters from the PCA performed on task data and conversation data (bottom rows). Participant number given at top of each panel. g, h, For 2 participants possessing mainly planning electrodes (Methods, Extended Data Table 1): bar graphs depicting the PC coefficients for all planning-related electrodes from the PCA performed on task data and conversation data. In the bar graphs, the functional categorization of PCs is indicated by filled bars coloured either green (perception), blue (planning), or red (production). Any clusters rejected due to a high proportion (50%) of mixed electrodes are indicated with grey filled bars.
Extended Data Table 1 Participant information
Extended Data Table 2 DKT parcellation of all CI task-responsive electrodes
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Castellucci, G.A., Kovach, C.K., Howard, M.A. et al. A speech planning network for interactive language use.Nature 602, 117–122 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04270-z
- Received: 29 September 2020
- Accepted: 19 November 2021
- Published: 05 January 2022
- Issue Date: 03 February 2022
- DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04270-z