Behavioral norms for condensed moral vignettes (original) (raw)

Journal Article

,

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Search for other works by this author on:

Search for other works by this author on:

*These authors contributed equally to this work.

Author Notes

Accepted:

24 November 2009

Published:

12 February 2010

Cite

Kristine M. Knutson, Frank Krueger, Michael Koenigs, Angelina Hawley, Jessica R. Escobedo, Viren Vasudeva, Ralph Adolphs, Jordan Grafman, Behavioral norms for condensed moral vignettes, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Volume 5, Issue 4, December 2010, Pages 378–384, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq005
Close

Navbar Search Filter Mobile Enter search term Search

Abstract

Moral judgment is an evaluation of the actions and character of a person made with respect to societal norms. Although many types of vignettes have been used in previous studies on moral beliefs and judgment, what is missing is a set of standardized common vignettes based in real life. The goal of this study was to provide researchers with stimuli that have values on several dimensions pertaining to moral judgment and whose underlying components are known. These values will allow researchers to select stimuli based on standardized ratings rather than on the results of pilot studies, while avoiding the limitations of the classic, abstract moral scenarios. Our study was composed of three phases, (i) collecting and shortening the vignettes, (ii) obtaining ratings of the vignettes on several dimensions including emotional intensity, degree of social norm violation, and level of harm or benefit caused and (iii) determining the underlying components of the vignettes by performing a factor analysis. We found three components that accounted for most of the variance: norm violation, social affect and intention. The resulting vignettes can be used in future parametric studies on moral judgment in behavioral, neuropsychological and functional imaging experiments.

INTRODUCTION

Moral judgment is defined as an evaluation of the actions and character of a person made with respect to the norms and values established in a society (Haidt, 2003 ; Prehn et al. , 2008 ). Much of moral behavior may be relatively unique to humans, albeit with some precursors such as cooperation and aggression in apes (Kluver and Bucy, 1939 ; de Waal, 2002 ; Emery et al. , 2001 ; Machado et al. , 2008 ). Moral judgment and related moral decisions and actions are thought to have contributed substantially to the evolution of human culture and society. Greene and Haidt ( 2002 ) suggested that morality evolved from humans’ expanding cognitive abilities that allowed the development of within-group altruism or cheating, cooperation and norm-following behavior. Establishing such social norms served to promote group cohesion and presumably survival (Hawley, 2003 ).

Rusbult and Van Lange ( 1996 ) proposed three sources of interpersonal moral orientation: (i) interpersonal dispositions, which are person-specific inclinations to act in particular patterns and can include predispositions toward being competitive, individualistic or prosocial; (ii) relationship-specific motives which are inclinations to respond in a specific manner within a particular relationship and (iii) social norms, which are rule-based inclinations to respond in a specific manner. Van Lange ( 2000 ) agreed that self-interest is too limited to fully explain the ways we behave and interact, and that prosocial forces (such as compassion) need to be considered. Many moral emotions such as guilt and compassion seem to have relevance only in a social context, and they often motivate behavior in the interests of long-term benefits of the social group rather than the short-term interests of only one person (Adolphs, 2003 ). Similar to Rusbult and Van Lange ( 1996 ), Shweder et al. ( 1997 ) described three ethical domains: (i) autonomy or ethics that protect the individual human being, including rights, justice, fairness and freedom; (ii) community or ethics that protect the group or society, including duty, respect, loyalty, hierarchy and interdependence and (iii) divinity or ethics that protect the spiritual aspects of the human and nature including sanctity and tradition. Haidt et al. (Haidt, 2007 ; Haidt and Graham, 2007 ) claimed that there are five moral foundations: (i) harm/care which includes virtues such as kindness and compassion, (ii) fairness/reciprocity, (iii) ingroup/loyalty, (iv) authority/respect and (v) purity/sanctity. These five moral foundations extend Shweder’s three ethical domains in that harm/care and fairness/reciprocity fit into the autonomy domain, ingroup/loyalty and authority/respect fit in with the domain of community and purity/sanctity fits in with the domain of divinity. The above theories describe the types and range of moral concerns. What we are looking to accomplish in this article is to make available a set of standardized common moral vignettes based in real life that attempts to cover the range of moral concerns in order to provide researchers with stimuli that have values on several dimensions pertaining to moral judgment and whose underlying components are known.

Different moral judgment studies have used different types of moral stimuli. As the study of moral judgment has been traditionally based in the domain of philosophy, many investigators (for example, Greene et al. , 2001 ; Koenigs et al. , 2007 ) have used complex dilemmas similar to those discussed by contemporary moral philosophers. These include the trolley scenario’ where if you do nothing, the trolley will proceed to the left, causing the deaths of five workmen, but if you hit a switch, causing the trolley to proceed to the right, it will result in the death of a single workman (Foot, 1978 ; Thomson, 1976 , 1985 , 1986). Increasingly, rather than approaching the study of moral judgment through a philosophical approach, cognitive neuroscience investigations are providing a powerful empirical approach to understanding this topic. Some of the types of stimuli used by cognitive neuroscience investigators have been short sentences containing either social norm violations or grammatical errors (for example, Heekeren et al. , 2003 ; Prehn et al. , 2008 ). Others used simple moral claims (For example, ‘They hung an innocent’; Moll, Eslinger and Oliveira-Souza, 2001 ), pictures with moral content (Moll et al. , 2002a ), or sentences with moral components (Moll et al. , 2002b ). Personal vs impersonal moral vignettes have been contrasted in studies (Greene et al. , 2001 , 2004 ; Koenigs et al. , 2007 ), and highly structured moral scenarios not based on real life experiences have been used (for example, Young et al. , 2007 ). Some researchers have used scenarios written in the first-person, while others used scenarios written in the third-person (including Berthoz et al. , 2006 ; Borg et al. , 2006 ).

Furthermore, different aspects of moral judgment have been studied, including the moral emotions such as compassion and guilt (Greene and Haidt, 2002 ), and scenarios involving direct vs indirect physical harm (Greene et al. , 2001 ; Greene and Haidt, 2002 ) and harm or no harm (Heekeren et al. , 2005 ). It is likely that these and other dimensions of moral behavior, including whether the behavior involves only oneself or others (socialness), the intensity of the emotions aroused from the behavior, the level of aversion and moral appropriateness, who benefits or is harmed because of the act, and whether the act is premeditated or impulsive, intentional or accidental, will have differential effects in functional neuroimaging experiments or patient studies [e.g. see Borg _et al._ ( 2006 )].

As in other domains of cognitive and social psychology, we argue it would be advantageous to researchers working in the area of moral cognition to have available a set of standardized stimuli in the form of vignettes containing elements of moral decision making. Therefore, we first collected moral judgment ratings for condensed versions of rather long, self-reported moral vignettes [see Escobedo ( 2009 )] on several dimensions including emotional intensity, degree of social norm violation and level of harm or benefit caused. Further, we analyzed the vignettes to determine their underlying cognitive and social–emotional components.

Using vignettes which are based on real life experiences and hence have ecological validity is important because moral cognition strongly depends on situational and cultural contexts (Casebeer, 2003 ) and in real life, moral judgment is often quick and implicit (Moll et al. , 2005 ). Basing moral judgments on extreme and unfamiliar situations such as those posed by classic moral dilemmas could evoke unusual strategies and thought processes rather than those typically used for common moral judgments (Moll et al. , 2005 ; Escobedo, 2009 ). Real-life scenarios are also more relevant to participants in studies and are more likely to be grounded in one’s personal upbringing, experiences and religious beliefs making it more likely that participants will be directly accessing knowledge stores related to established moral rules and behavior.

METHODS

Subjects

Thirty normal healthy adult volunteers (15 females and 15 males, mean ± s.d.: age 26.7 ± 5.1, with 17 ± 2.4 years of education) participated in the normative study performed at the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) in Bethesda, MD. There were no significant sex differences in age [females: 27.1 ± 4.8; males: 26.3 ± 5.5; t (28) = 0.43, P = 0.67] or education [females: 17.6 ± 2.3; males: 16.5 ± 2.5; t (28) = 1.31, P = 0.20]. Participants signed a consent form approved by the NINDS Institutional Review Board and were paid for their participation. Participants reported no prior histories of neurological or psychiatric disorders or learning disabilities, and were not taking any antidepressant or psychotropic medications at the time of testing.

Procedure

Our study was composed of three phases: (i) collecting and shortening the vignettes, (ii) obtaining ratings of the vignettes and (iii) determining the underlying moral cognitive components of the vignettes by performing a factor analysis.

Phase 1: Collecting and shortening the vignettes

Our vignettes are based on those compiled by Escobedo et al. ( 2009 ), who collected 758 first-person moral vignettes based on episodic memories that were solicited using cue words. Their cue words were selected from a set of potential cues generated by two of the authors (J.R.E. and R.A.), who chose three types of cue words: emotions, actions and superlatives. The emotion-type cues were chosen to span the valence spectrum and included three positive (proud, compassionate and virtuous), four intermediate (responsible, relieved, bittersweet and doubtful) and three negative cues (regretful, embarrassed and guilty). The action cues (honest, tempted, qualms, reckless, sneaky, hurtful, cheated, lied, took something and unfaithful) also were chosen to elicit both positive and negative moral experiences. The superlative cues ask for some of the best and worst events in someone’s life. There also were four control words, two of which, happy and tired, elicited a few moral experiences.

Using these cues, Escobedo et al. ( 2009 ) collected the moral vignettes from a representative sample of 100 English-speaking healthy adults (47 male) who had resided in Southern California for at least the last 15 years, were recruited using Craigslist, and were between the ages of 40 and 60 (mean age 48.9, s.d. 5.9 years). Participants were excluded if they were under the care of a neurologist or had any history of head injury or seizures, tumor or brain surgery, if they had been diagnosed with any major psychiatric illness or were taking any medications for psychiatric conditions, were unemployed or homeless or had an IQ under 80. The participants held a mix of religious and political beliefs, and matched the ethnic diversity and IQ (mean 110) of California as specified in the US census 2000. Their median income in 2004 was $30 000–60 000. Escobedo et al. ( 2009 ) verified the truthfulness of these vignettes via follow-up phone calls 2 years later to eleven of the original participants and found that all 11 were able to reproduce narratives that matched their original ones. After collecting the moral vignettes, Escobedo et al. ( 2009 ), using a list of the original cues, had a separate set of 55 raters select all of the cues that applied to each of the vignettes by circling them in a list. These raters were all affiliated with the California Institute of Technology as either alumni or colleagues. No further demographics are available for these raters.

For the present study, we condensed each of these moral vignettes, which originally averaged 218 words, into moral vignettes of two or three sentences. This was performed so that they could easily be used as experimental stimuli, since long stories that require significant time to read and comprehend would be unfeasible in many patient and functional neuroimaging studies, and since stories varying widely in length would have story length as a confounding variable. Some stories were determined to be too long and complicated to reasonably condense and so were removed from the database. Next, we eliminated those stories that did not have a main cue; that is, a single cue applied to a story more than any other cue by Escobedo et al. ’s 55 raters (2009). This was performed so that researchers could select vignettes to use as stimuli for studies based on the main cue used to solicit the vignette.

Phase 2: Obtaining ratings

We next had our 30 normal healthy adults rate the 312 vignettes on 13 dimensions (emotional intensity, emotional aversion, harm, self-benefit, other-benefit, pre-meditation, illegality, social norm violations, socialness, frequency, personal familiarity, general familiarity and moral appropriateness). Ratings were taken using a computer-based survey shown in Figure 1 .

Screen shots of the questionnaire given to participants for rating the vignettes on 13 dimensions using a scale from 1 to 7. Each vignette appeared in the box at the top.

Fig. 1

Screen shots of the questionnaire given to participants for rating the vignettes on 13 dimensions using a scale from 1 to 7. Each vignette appeared in the box at the top.

Phase 3: Factor analysis

We then performed a factor analysis using SPSS version 11 with the option for Varimax rotation and Kaiser normalization (Kaiser, 1958 ) on the vignettes using the ratings from 10 of our dimensions (excluding the dimensions of frequency, personal and general familiarity as these were not direct ratings of aspects of morality, socialness or emotionality). The purpose of the factor analysis was to reduce these dimensions to describe the underlying moral components of the vignettes. The Anderson–Rubin method (Anderson and Rubin, 1956 ) was used to calculate the factor score coefficients; this is a modification of the Bartlett method (Bartlett, 1937 ) and ensures orthogonality of the estimated factors. The resulting scores have a mean of 0 and a s.d. of 1.

RESULTS

Collecting and shortening the vignettes

The 312 moral vignettes were condensed into vignettes of two or three sentences averaging 43 words (and with a limited range of 28–59 words). The complete database of the moral vignettes is available as a Supplement . This database includes the word counts for each vignette and the main cue Escobedo’s raters applied to it most frequently.

Obtaining ratings

Each vignette’s ratings on the 13 dimensions (emotional intensity, emotional aversion, harm, self-benefit, other-benefit, pre-meditation, illegality, social norm violations, socialness, frequency, personal familiarity, general familiarity and moral appropriateness) are listed in the Supplement . All the dimensions including frequency, personal and general familiarity are included so that researchers can choose vignettes with equivalent (or varying) ratings depending on their needs.

Factor analysis

The factor analysis on the vignettes resulted in three components with an eigenvalue greater than 1.0 ( Figure 2 ). The various indicators of factorability were acceptable, and the residuals indicate that the solution was acceptable. We interpret the components to represent: (i) norm violation, (ii) social affect and (iii) intention. The norm violation component was most positively correlated with the dimensions of social norm violation, harm and illegality, and negatively with other benefit and moral appropriateness. It explained 40% of the variance. The social affect component was most positively correlated with emotional intensity, socialness and emotional aversion. It explained 24% of the variance. The third component, intention, was most positively correlated with premeditation and self-benefit. It explained 15% of the variance. Table 1 shows the three components and the mean ratings on the dimensions for each. The scores for each vignette on the norm violation, social affect and intention factors are included in the Supplement .

Screen plot resulting from the factor analysis showing three components with eigenvalues >1.

Fig. 2

Screen plot resulting from the factor analysis showing three components with eigenvalues >1.

Table 1

Results of the factor analysis

Components Dimensions Norm violation Social affect Intention
1. Norm violation Social norm violation 0.947 0.154 0.144
Harm 0.803 0.473 0.009
Illegality 0.737 −0.288 0.115
Other benefit −0.883 0.046 0.051
Moral appropriateness −0.956 −0.102 −0.120
2. Social affect Emotional intensity 0.024 0.896 −0.066
Socialness −0.115 0.763 0.154
Emotional aversion 0.336 0.762 −0.258
3. Intention Premeditation −0.002 0.175 0.859
Self-benefit 0.244 −0.304 0.772
Components Dimensions Norm violation Social affect Intention
1. Norm violation Social norm violation 0.947 0.154 0.144
Harm 0.803 0.473 0.009
Illegality 0.737 −0.288 0.115
Other benefit −0.883 0.046 0.051
Moral appropriateness −0.956 −0.102 −0.120
2. Social affect Emotional intensity 0.024 0.896 −0.066
Socialness −0.115 0.763 0.154
Emotional aversion 0.336 0.762 −0.258
3. Intention Premeditation −0.002 0.175 0.859
Self-benefit 0.244 −0.304 0.772

Table 1

Results of the factor analysis

Components Dimensions Norm violation Social affect Intention
1. Norm violation Social norm violation 0.947 0.154 0.144
Harm 0.803 0.473 0.009
Illegality 0.737 −0.288 0.115
Other benefit −0.883 0.046 0.051
Moral appropriateness −0.956 −0.102 −0.120
2. Social affect Emotional intensity 0.024 0.896 −0.066
Socialness −0.115 0.763 0.154
Emotional aversion 0.336 0.762 −0.258
3. Intention Premeditation −0.002 0.175 0.859
Self-benefit 0.244 −0.304 0.772
Components Dimensions Norm violation Social affect Intention
1. Norm violation Social norm violation 0.947 0.154 0.144
Harm 0.803 0.473 0.009
Illegality 0.737 −0.288 0.115
Other benefit −0.883 0.046 0.051
Moral appropriateness −0.956 −0.102 −0.120
2. Social affect Emotional intensity 0.024 0.896 −0.066
Socialness −0.115 0.763 0.154
Emotional aversion 0.336 0.762 −0.258
3. Intention Premeditation −0.002 0.175 0.859
Self-benefit 0.244 −0.304 0.772

DISCUSSION

The goal of this study was to provide researchers with vignettes based on real life that have values on 13 dimensions pertaining to moral judgment and whose underlying moral components are known. We found three components:

Haidt et al. , claim that morality is composed of ‘five sets of intuitions that human minds are prepared to have’ and that these five moral foundations are not separate modules (Haidt and Graham, 2007 ). The cues used to elicit our vignettes represent behaviors or emotions that fit into one or more of these foundations. For example, the ‘hurtful to someone’ and ‘compassion’ cues are represented in Haidt et al. ’s theory by their harm/care foundation. Also the cues of ‘guilt’ and ‘took something’ fit into the fairness/reciprocity foundation; the cue of ‘unfaithful’ fits into both the ingroup/loyalty and the fairness/reciprocity foundations; and the cue of ‘sneaky’ contains components of several foundations including the harm/care, fairness/reciprocity and authority/respect foundations. Of course, there are other concerns such as emotional intensity and aversion, intention of the actor and whether or not the transgression was carried out or only considered. These are covered by the second and third components we found underlying the structure of our moral vignettes, social affect and intentionality.

The original cues used by Escobedo et al. ( 2009 ) to elicit the moral vignettes certainly influenced the outcome, resulting in vignettes containing a range of moral emotions and actions. However, as Escobedo et al. ( 2009 ) eliminated vignettes that based their morality on a reference to a religious belief, keeping only those vignettes that based their morality on a personal moral code, the resulting vignettes do not include any dealing with divinity or religious belief. For this reason, there are no vignettes that fit into Shweder et al. ’s ethical domain of divinity (1997) or Haidt’s moral foundation of purity/sanctity (Haidt, 2007 ; Haidt and Graham, 2007 ). This limitation within our vignettes should be kept in mind when discussing the results of studies using these vignettes. A further limitation is that the original stories were rated by a highly educated group of college alumni and colleagues, and the raters in the present study also were highly educated and fairly young (mean age 26.7). We therefore encourage other researchers to collect ratings on these vignettes using additional demographic groups.

In summary, the set of 312 moral vignettes reflects a variety of common moral behaviors and complements the moral scenarios used by others (for example, Greene et al. , 2001 ; Moll, et al. , 2001 ; Moll et al. , 2002a ; Moll et al. , 2002b ; Greene and Haidt, 2002 ; Heekeren et al. , 2003 ; Greene et al. , 2004 ; Heekeren et al. , 2005 ; Koenigs et al. , 2007 ; Berthoz et al. , 2006 ; Borg, et al. , 2006 ; Young et al. , 2007 ; Prehn et al. , 2008 ). In our opinion, using moral vignettes based on real life will avoid the inherent characteristic involved in judging classic complex and impersonal scenarios like the trolley scenario (Foot, 1978 ; Thomson, 1976 , 1985 , 1986), in which abstract reasoning and problem solving are required in addition to processes usually used in common moral judgment. We believe it is important to identify the underlying moral components of the vignettes, as they will likely affect the results of brain imaging studies or patient studies (Borg, 2008 ). Our vignettes show a range of values across the norm violation, social affect and intention components. These well-described vignettes will facilitate research on moral judgment and cognition since they are based on real life, are short so can be read quickly during a behavioral, neuropsychological or functional imaging experiment, have a limited range of word length, are each based on one main cue, and are well characterized both by ratings and the component scores. Given the improvement in theorizing in psycholinguistics, object recognition, sentence processing and other psychological domains that came with the availability of normed stimuli, we hope that the use of our vignettes will provide a similar boost to studies of moral judgment and social neuroscience in general (Moll et al. , 2005 ; Adolphs, 2009 ).

This study was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health .

REFERENCES

Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour

,

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

,

2003

, vol.

4

(pg.

165

-

78

)

The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge

,

Annual Review of Psychology

,

2009

, vol.

60

(pg.

693

-

716

)

Statistical inference in factor analysis

,

Proceedings of the Third Berkeley Symposium of Mathematical Statistics and Probability

,

1956

, vol.

5

(pg.

111

-

50

)

The statistical conception of mental factors

,

British Journal of Psychology

,

1937

, vol.

28

(pg.

97

-

104

)

Affective response to one's; own moral violations

,

Neuroimage

,

2006

, vol.

31

(pg.

945

-

50

)

Infection, incest, and iniquity: Investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality

,

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

,

2008

, vol.

20

9

(pg.

1529

-

46

)

Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments: an FMRI investigation

,

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

,

2006

, vol.

18

5

(pg.

803

-

17

)

Moral cognition and its neural constituents

,

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

,

2003

, vol.

4

(pg.

840

-

6

)

,

2002

Tree of Origin: What Primate Behavior Can Tell Us About Human Social Evolution . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys ( Macaca mulatta )

,

Behavioral Neuroscience

,

2001

, vol.

115

(pg.

515

-

44

)

,

Investigating Moral Events: Characterization and Structure of Autobiographical Moral Memories. Unpublished Dissertation

,

2009

Pasadena, California

California Institute of Technology

,

The Problem of Abortion and the Doctrine of the Double Effect in Virtues and Vices and Other Essays in Moral Philosophy

,

1978

Berkeley and Los Angeles

University of California Press

How (and where) does moral judgment work?

,

Trends in Cognitive Sciences

,

2002

, vol.

6

12

(pg.

517

-

23

)

The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment

,

Neuron

,

2004

, vol.

44

(pg.

389

-

400

)

An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment

,

Science

,

2001

, vol.

293

(pg.

2105

-

8

)

The moral emotions

,

Handbook of Affective Sciences

,

2003

Oxford

Oxford University Press

(pg.

852

-

70

)

The new synthesis in moral psychology

,

Science

,

2007

, vol.

316

(pg.

998

-

1002

)

When morality opposes justice: conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize

,

Social Justice Research

,

2007

, vol.

20

1

(pg.

98

-

116

)

Strategies of control, aggression, and morality in preschoolers: an evolutionary perspective

,

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

,

2003

, vol.

85

(pg.

213

-

35

)

Influence of bodily harm on neural correlates of semantic and moral decision-making

,

Neuroimage

,

2005

, vol.

24

(pg.

887

-

97

)

An fMRI study of simple ethical decision-making

,

Neuroreport

,

2003

, vol.

14

9

(pg.

1215

-

9

)

The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis

,

Psychometrika

,

1958

, vol.

23

(pg.

187

-

200

)

Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys

,

Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry

,

1939

, vol.

42

(pg.

979

-

97

)

et al.

Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements

,

Nature

,

2007

, vol.

446

(pg.

908

-

11

)

Bilateral neurotoxic amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys ( Macaca mulatta ): consistent pattern of behavior across different social contexts

,

Behavioral Neuroscience

,

2008

, vol.

122

2

(pg.

251

-

66

)

Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments

,

NeuroImage

,

2002

, vol.

16

(pg.

696

-

703

)

et al.

The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions

,

Journal of Neuroscience

,

2002

, vol.

22

7

(pg.

2730

-

6

)

Frontopolar and anterior temporal cortex activation in a moral judgment task: preliminary functional MRI results in normal subjects

,

Arq Neuro-psiquiatr

,

2001

, vol.

59

3-B

(pg.

657

-

64

)

Opinion: the neural basis of human moral cognition

,

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

,

2005

, vol.

6

10

(pg.

799

-

809

)

et al.

Individual differences in moral judgment competence influence neural correlates of socio-normative judgments

,

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

,

2008

, vol.

3

(pg.

33

-

46

)

Interdependence processes

,

Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles

,

1996

New York

Guilford

(pg.

564

-

96

)

The "Big Three" of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the "Big Three" explanations of suffering

,

Morality and Health

,

1997

New York

Routledge

(pg.

119

-

69

)

Killing, letting die, and the trolley problem

,

The Monist

,

1976

, vol.

59

(pg.

204

-

217

)

The trolley problem

,

Yale Law Journal

,

1985

, vol.

94

(pg.

1395

-

415

)

,

Rights, Restitution, and Risk: Essays in Moral Theory

,

1986

Cambridge, MA

Harvard University Press

Beyond self-interest: a set of propositions relevant to interpersonal orientations

,

2000

European Review of Social Psychology , Vol. 11. London: Wiley, pp. 297–331

The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment

,

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

,

2007

, vol.

104

20

(pg.

8235

-

40

)

Author notes

*These authors contributed equally to this work.

© The Author(s) (2010). Published by Oxford University Press.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact journals.permissions@oup.com

Citations

Views

Altmetric

Metrics

Total Views 4,240

2,957 Pageviews

1,283 PDF Downloads

Since 1/1/2017

Month: Total Views:
January 2017 9
February 2017 19
March 2017 25
April 2017 13
May 2017 22
June 2017 12
July 2017 10
August 2017 21
September 2017 25
October 2017 12
November 2017 25
December 2017 35
January 2018 39
February 2018 63
March 2018 56
April 2018 52
May 2018 56
June 2018 43
July 2018 47
August 2018 26
September 2018 69
October 2018 69
November 2018 55
December 2018 52
January 2019 35
February 2019 60
March 2019 94
April 2019 51
May 2019 67
June 2019 45
July 2019 53
August 2019 57
September 2019 45
October 2019 65
November 2019 68
December 2019 60
January 2020 69
February 2020 79
March 2020 51
April 2020 18
May 2020 18
June 2020 49
July 2020 43
August 2020 25
September 2020 63
October 2020 74
November 2020 44
December 2020 46
January 2021 51
February 2021 32
March 2021 56
April 2021 76
May 2021 62
June 2021 56
July 2021 37
August 2021 28
September 2021 46
October 2021 56
November 2021 66
December 2021 41
January 2022 21
February 2022 43
March 2022 35
April 2022 71
May 2022 82
June 2022 45
July 2022 25
August 2022 49
September 2022 41
October 2022 102
November 2022 24
December 2022 42
January 2023 51
February 2023 45
March 2023 68
April 2023 75
May 2023 36
June 2023 42
July 2023 32
August 2023 30
September 2023 36
October 2023 45
November 2023 33
December 2023 24
January 2024 61
February 2024 25
March 2024 33
April 2024 58
May 2024 37
June 2024 25
July 2024 30
August 2024 30
September 2024 32
October 2024 49
November 2024 22

Citations

39 Web of Science

×

Email alerts

Citing articles via

More from Oxford Academic