Cognitive control and parsing: Reexamining the role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension (original) (raw)
References
Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., Bürgel, U., Mohlberg, H., Uylings, H. B. M., & Zilles, K. (1999). Broca’s region revisited: Cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 412, 319–341. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. [D.] (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
Boland, J. E., & Blodgett, A. (2001). Understanding the constraints on syntactic generation: Lexical bias and discourse congruency effects on eye movements. Journal of Memory & Language, 45, 391–411. Article Google Scholar
Boland, J. E., & Cutler, A. (1996). Interaction with autonomy: Multiple output models and the inadequacy of the Great Divide. Cognition, 58, 309–320. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Selection for cognitive control: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study on the selection of task-relevant information. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 8847–8852. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Britt, M. A. (1994). The interaction of referential ambiguity and argument structure in the parsing of prepositional phrases. Journal of Memory & Language, 33, 251–283. Article Google Scholar
Broca, P. P. (1861). Loss of speech, chronic softening and partial destruction of the anterior left lobe of the brain (Christopher D. Green, Trans.). Retrieved September 10, 2003, from http://psychclassics. yorku.ca. Bulletin de la Société Anthropologique, 2, 235–238. Google Scholar
Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33, 1–11. Article Google Scholar
Campbell, R. N., & Bowe, T. (1983). Text and context in early language comprehension. In M. Donaldson, R. Grieve, & C. Pratt (Eds.), Early childhood development and education (pp. 115–126). Oxford: Blackwell. Google Scholar
Caplan, D. (1995). Issues arising in contemporary studies of disorders of syntactic processing in sentence comprehension in agrammatic patients. Brain & Language, 50, 325–338. Article Google Scholar
Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G., & Olivieri, A. (2000). Activation of Broca’s area by syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Human Brain Mapping, 9, 65–71. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. (2002). Working memory and connectionist models of parsing: A reply to MacDonald and Christiansen (2002). Psychological Review, 109, 66–74. Article Google Scholar
Carlson, G. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1988). Thematic roles and language comprehension. In W. Wilkins (Ed.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 21. Thematic relations (pp. 263–289). London: Academic Press. Google Scholar
Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 280, 747–749. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Clifton, C., Traxler, M. J., Mohamed, M. T., Williams, R. S., Morris, R. K., & Rayner, K. (2003). The use of thematic role information in parsing: Syntactic processing autonomy revisited. Journal of Memory & Language, 49, 317–334. Article Google Scholar
Damian, M. F., Vigliocco, G., & Levelt, W. J. (2001). Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words. Cognition, 81, 77–86. Article Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. Article Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Posner, M. I., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Science, 5, 303–305. Google Scholar
D’Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (1999). The dependence of span and delayed-response performance on prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1303–1315. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Diamond, A., & Doar, B. (1989). The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 271–294. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1997). Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to “Do as I say, not as I do.” Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 315–334. Article Google Scholar
Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, B., Utman, J., Dronkers, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2001). Language deficits, localization, and grammar: Evidence for a distributive model of language breakdown in aphasic patients and neurologically intact individuals. Psychological Review, 108, 759–788. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Doherty, M. J. (2004). Children’s difficulty in learning homonyms. Journal of Child Language, 31, 203–214. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., Van Valin, R. D., Jr., Redfern, B. B., & Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 92, 145–177. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Durston, S., Thomas, K. M., Yang, Y., Uluĝ, A. M., Zimmerman, R. D., & Casey, B. J. (2002). A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 5, F9-F16. Article Google Scholar
Ferreira, F., Christianson, K., & Hollingworth, A. (2001). Misinterpretations of garden-path sentences: Implications for models of reanalysis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 3–20. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1990). The use of verb information in syntactic parsing: A comparison of evidence from eye movements and self-paced word-by-word reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 16, 555–568. Article Google Scholar
Fiebach, C. J., Vos, S. H., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic ambiguity in sentence comprehension for low and high span readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1562–1575. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Fletcher, P. C., Shallice, T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). Brain activity during memory retrieval: The influence of imagery and semantic cueing. Brain, 119, 1587–1596. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Murphy, K., Roche, R. A. P., & Stein, E. A. (2002). Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: Inhibition, error detection, and correction. NeuroImage, 17, 1820–1829. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Garnsey, S. M., Pearlmutter, N. J., Myers, E., & Lotocky, M. A. (1997). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory & Language, 37, 58–93. Article Google Scholar
Gehring, W. J., & Knight, R. T. (2000). Prefrontal—cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 516–520. Article Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1993). Less skilled readers have less efficient suppression mechanisms. Psychological Science, 4, 294–298. Article Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Robertson, R. (1995). Reading skill and suppression revisited. Psychological Science, 6, 165–169. Article Google Scholar
Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 1–76. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca’s area. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 23, 1–71. Article Google Scholar
Hagoort, P. (2003). How the brain solves the binding problem for language. Manuscript from a position paper presented at the 9th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing, Glasgow, Scotland.
Hagoort, P., Wassenaar, M., & Brown, C. (2003). Real-time semantic compensation in patients with agrammatic comprehension: Electrophysiological evidence for multiple-route plasticity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 4340–4345. Article Google Scholar
Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2005). Dissociations among tasks involving inhibition: A single-case study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 1–13. Article Google Scholar
Hurewitz, F., Brown-Schmidt, S., Thorpe, K., Gleitman, L. R., & Trueswell, J. C. (2001). One frog, two frog, red frog, blue frog: Factors affecting children’s syntactic choices in production and comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 597–626. Article Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 167–178. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Indefrey, P., Hellwig, F., Herzog, H., Seitz, R. J., & Hagoort, P. (2004). Neural responses to the production and comprehension of syntax in identical utterances. Brain & Language, 89, 312–319. Article Google Scholar
Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R. A., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 8410–8413. Article Google Scholar
Joshi, A. K., & Schabes, Y. (1997). Tree-adjoining grammars. In G. Rozenberg & A. Salomaa (Eds.), Handbook of formal languages (pp. 69–124). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Keller, T. A. (1996). The capacity theory of comprehension: New frontiers of evidence and arguments. Psychological Review, 103, 773–780. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y. (2002). The brain circuitry of syntactic comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 350–356. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A.W., III, Cho, R.Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate, conflict monitoring, and adjustments in control. Science, 303, 1023–1026. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Kim, A. E., Srinivas, B., & Trueswell, J. C. (2002). The convergence of lexicalist perspectives in psycholinguistics and computational linguistics. In P. Merlo & S. Stevenson (Eds.), The lexical basis of sentence processing: Formal, computational, and experimental perspectives (pp. 109–135). Philadelphia: Benjamins. Google Scholar
Lewis, R. (1996). Interference in short-term memory: The magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 93–115. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Luciana, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1998). The functional emergence of prefrontally guided working memory systems in four- to eight-yearold children. Neuropsychologia, 36, 273–293. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. New York: Basic Books. Google Scholar
MacDonald, A.W., III, Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C. (1994). Probabilistic constraints and syntactic ambiguity resolution. Language & Cognitive Processes, 9, 157–201. Article Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., & Christiansen, M. (2002). Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychological Review, 109, 35–54. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676–703. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Mason, R. A., Just, M. A., Keller, T. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (2003). Ambiguity in the brain: What brain imaging reveals about the processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 29, 1319–1338. Article Google Scholar
Mazzocco, M. M. M. (1997). Children’s interpretations of homonyms: A developmental study. Journal of Child Language, 24, 441–467. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
McElree, B. M., & Dosher, B. A. (1989). Serial position and set size in short-term memory: The time course of recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 346–373. Article Google Scholar
McRae, K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in online sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 38, 283–312. Article Google Scholar
Mendelsohn, A. (2002). Individual differences in ambiguity resolution: Working memory and inhibition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northeastern University.
Metzler, C. (2001). Effects of left frontal lesions on the selection of context-appropriate meanings. Neuropsychology, 15, 315–328. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Milham, M. P., Banich, M. T., & Barad, V. (2003). Competition for priority in processing increases prefrontal cortex’s involvement in top-down control: An event-related fMRI study of the Stroop task. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 212–222. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Milham, M. P., Banich, M. T., Webb, A., Barad, V., Cohen, N. J., Wszalek, T., & Kramer, A. F. (2001). The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 467–473. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1994). Working memory constraints on the resolution of lexical ambiguity: Maintaining multiple interpretations in neutral contexts. Journal of Memory & Language, 33, 175–202. Article Google Scholar
Monsell, S. (1978). Recency, immediate recognition memory, and reaction time. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 465–501. Article Google Scholar
Nathaniel-James, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (2002). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: Evidence from the effects of contextual constraint in a sentence completion task. NeuroImage, 16, 1094–1102. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Nelson, J. K., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Sylvester, C. C., Jonides, J., & Smith, E. E. (2003). Dissociable neural mechanisms underlying response-based and familiarity-based conflict in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 11171–11175. Article Google Scholar
Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28. Article Google Scholar
Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2004). An f MRI study of syntactic adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 702–713. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Novick, J. M., January, D., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2004). Prefrontal cortex and the role of selectional processes in language comprehension: Frogs, napkins, and Broca’s area. Poster presented at the 17th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Baltimore.
Novick, J. M., Kim, A., & Trueswell, J. C. (2003). Studying the grammatical aspects of word recognition: Lexical priming, parsing, and syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 57–75. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., January, D., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2004). The role of conflict resolution in parsing: Measuring individual differences in syntactic ambiguity resolution and executive control. Paper presented at the 10th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms in Language Processing, Aix-en-Provence, France.
Pearson, D. A., & Lane, D. M. (1991). Auditory attention switching: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 320–334. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature, 331, 985–989. Google Scholar
Pribram, K. H. (1997). The work in working memory: Implications for development. In N. A. Krasnegor & G. R. Lyon (Eds.), Development of the prefrontal cortex: Evolution, Neurobiology, and behavior (pp. 359–378). Baltimore: Brookes. Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Carlson, M., & Frazier, L. (1983). The interaction of syntax and semantics during sentence processing: Eye movements in the analysis of semantically biased sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 22, 358–374. Article Google Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–446. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Roberts, R., & Gibson, E. (2002). Individual differences in sentence memory. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 573–598. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Robinson, G., Blair, J., & Cipolotti, L. (1998). Dynamic aphasia: An inability to select between competing verbal responses? Brain, 121, 77–89. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 238–299. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Spivey, M., Tanenhaus, M., Eberhard, K., & Sedivy, J. (2002). Eye movements and spoken language comprehension: Effects of visual context on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 447–481. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Spivey-Knowlton, M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Resolving attachment ambiguities with multiple constraints. Cognition, 55, 227–267. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. Article Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Taraban, R., & McClelland, J. L. (1988). Constituent attachment and thematic role assignment in sentence processing: Influences of content-based expectations. Journal of Memory & Language, 27, 597–632. Article Google Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S. L., Bedny, M., & Goldberg, R. F. (2005). The frontal lobes and the regulation of mental activity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 219–224. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S. L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., & Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: A reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 14792–14797. Article Google Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S. L., Jonides, J., Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., D’Esposito,M., Kan, I. P., Knight, R. T., & Swick, D. (2002). Effects of frontal lobe damage on interference effects in working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 109–120. Article Google Scholar
Thompson-Schill, S. L., Swick, D., Farah, M. J., D’Esposito, M., Kan, I. P., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Verb generation in patients with focal frontal lesions: A neuropsychological test of neuroimaging findings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 15855–15860. Article Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. [C.], & Gleitman, L. R. (2004). Children’s eye movements during listening: Developmental evidence for a constraint-based theory of sentence processing. In J. M. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The interface of Language, vision, and action: Eye-movements and the visual world (pp. 319–346). New York: Taylor & Francis, Psychology Press. Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Papafragou, A., & Choi, Y. (in press). Syntactic and referential processes: What develops? In E. Gibson & N. Perlmutter (Eds.), The processing and acquisition of reference. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73, 89–134. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1994). Toward a lexicalist framework of constraint-based syntactic ambiguity resolution. In C. J. Clifton, L. Frazier, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Perspectives on sentence processing (pp. 155–179). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory & Language, 33, 285–318. Article Google Scholar
Vitkovitch, M., & Humphreys, G. W. (1991). Perseverant responding in speeded naming of pictures: It’s in the links. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 17, 664–680. Article Google Scholar
Warburton, E., Wise, R. J. S., Price, C. J., Weiller, C., Hadar, U., Ramsay, S., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1996). Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects: Studies with PET. Brain, 119, 159–179. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996). The capacity theory of sentence comprehension: Critique of Just and Carpenter (1992). Psychological Review, 103, 761–772. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Wilshire, C. E., & McCarthy, R. A. (2002). Evidence for a contextsensitive word retrieval disorder in a case of nonfluent aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 165–185. Article Google Scholar
Zhang, J. X., Feng, C. M., Fox, P. T., Gao, J. H., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Is left inferior frontal gyrus a general mechanism for selection? NeuroImage, 23, 596–603. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar
Zurif, E. B., Caramazza, A., & Myerson, R. (1972). Grammatical judgments of agrammatic aphasics. Neuropsychologia, 10, 405–417. ArticlePubMed Google Scholar